Skip to main content

Effects of repeated unihemispheric concurrent dual-site tDCS and virtual reality games on motor coordination of sedentary adolescent girls

Abstract

Background

This study investigated the effects of repetitive unihemispheric concurrent dual-site anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (a-tDCSUHCDS) associated with the use of virtual reality games (VR) on the motor coordination of sedentary adolescent girls.

Methods

Thirty-six inactive adolescent girls were randomly assigned into 3 groups (n = 12 per group): (1) VR + a-tDCSUHCDS, (2) VR + sham-tDCSUHCDS, and (3) Control. The VR + a-tDCSUHCDS and VR + s-tDCSUHCDS groups received the intervention three times a week for four weeks. In each experimental session, participants first received either 20 min of a-tDCSUHCDS (2 mA at each anodal electrode) targeting the primary motor cortex (M1) and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) or sham and then performed VR for 1 h. The control group received no intervention. Eye-hand coordination (EHC) and bimanual coordination (BC) were measured at baseline, post-intervention, and two weeks later (retention test) using the automatic scoring mirror tracer and continuous two-arm coordination test, respectively.

Results

Results showed that the EHC was significantly higher in the VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS groups at post-intervention (all ps< 0.001) and the retention test (all ps< 0.001) compared to the control group. Moreover, the EHC was significantly higher in the VR + a-tDCS group compared to the VR + s-tDCS group (p = 0.024) at the retention. Similarly, VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS improved BC compared to the control group at post-intervention (all ps< 0.001) and retention test (all ps< 0.001). In addition, higher BC was observed in the VR + a-tDCS group compared to the VR + s-tDCS group (p< 0.001) at the retention test.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that adding a-tDCSUHCDS to VR over 12 sessions may have an additional effect on VR training for improving and retaining motor coordination in sedentary adolescent girls.

Background

During adolescence, a considerable proportion of leisure time is spent engaged in sedentary activities, which are defined as waking behaviors characterized by tasks with low energy expenditure (< 1.5 metabolic equivalents of task; MET) [1]. Given the significance of the physical, emotional, mental, and developmental changes that occur during adolescence, regular physical activity (PA) can facilitate the development of an active lifestyle and, to some extent, ensure appropriate participation in future activities [2, 3]. It is therefore recommended that adolescents engage in at least 60 min of moderate to vigorous PA daily [4]. However, recent reports have indicated that approximately 77.6% of boys and 84.7% of girls aged 11 to 17 years do not engage in the recommended amount of PA [5]. Furthermore, the levels of PA exhibit a notable decline during the adolescent period [6].

Coordination abilities are among the essential needs for physical skills that allow a person to use neuromuscular and motor coordination of the muscles and body segments/limbs for a successful and accurate execution of any motor task [7]. In particular, there is evidence of a reciprocal effect between PA and coordination abilities, whereby lack of coordination may contribute to a reduced willingness to engage in PA, and such a decrease in PA negatively affects coordination abilities [8]. Eye-hand coordination (EHC) is the ability to perform activities that require the simultaneous use of both hands and eyes to collect information from the visual field and to guide the hands to achieve a desired outcome. [9]. EHC is a fundamental component of numerous daily activities, including but not limited to tool use, food preparation, exercise, and work [10]. Normal EHC involves the synergistic functioning of several sensorimotor systems, including the visual system, vestibular system, proprioception, and eye, head, and arm control systems, as well as attention and other cognitive aspects [7]. Studies have shown that early acquisition of the EHC skill leads to improvements in fitness, body image, and participation in sport [11].

Bimanual coordination (BC) is another vital aspect of coordination skills, which is very important for performing daily activities and also for engaging in PA [12]. BC skills require a well-coordinated interplay between the upper extremities, which is controlled by several brain areas such as the primary motor cortex (M1), supplementary motor cortex, premotor cortex, cingulate and posterior parietal cortex, and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [13,14,15]. Successful performance of bimanual tasks depends not only on the spatio-temporal synchrony of the movements of the two hands but also on visual feedback and EHC patterns [16]. This is particularly evident during the terminal phase of a bimanual task when temporal symmetry breaks down and different patterns of EHC emerge [16]. This shows the link between EHC and BC and their interplay in coordination abilities.

Considering the reciprocal effect between PA and coordination skills, the development of new strategies to increase engagement in the PA and also enhance coordination abilities such as EHC and BC has received attention. Virtual reality (VR) is a relatively new technology in which a simulated experience is created to give users an immersive feel of a virtual world [17]. Sport VR games have recently been recognized as a new strategy to increase PA and promote health benefits in healthy and clinical populations [18]. It has also been reported that sports VR games improve reaction time, EHC, motor control, and motor coordination, probably by enhancing visual perception and facilitating visual feedback cues in the environment [19,20,21,22]. Given the importance of early adolescence (12–17 years) as a critical period of growth and a cornerstone for establishing a healthy lifestyle, VR may be a promising intervention strategy for engaging adolescents in physical activity and improving motor function [23]. Recently, VR and other so-called “exergames” (i.e., video games that involve physical exertion) have been widely used by children and adolescents, and this new generation of games may help to increase PA and reduce sedentary lifestyles [24]. VR increases the amount of energy that children and adolescents spend on video games and allows them to increase their PA, which also has the potential to overcome barriers to participation in real-world PA [25]. Recent evidence suggested positive effects of VR on motor function in children and adolescents [26,27,28]. Wing et al. [26] reported an improvement in EHC with VR among elementary school children. Caro et al. [27] also investigated the effects of VR on improving eye-body coordination in children with autism and found that children with severe autism kept their attention for the entire duration of treatment, reduced aimless limb movements, and improved limb movements as a result of exergaming. They also found that exergaming can be effective for children with autism in improving eye-body coordination, especially in helping to develop limb movements [27].

In addition, many researchers are trying to design and devise new intervention methods to make training sessions more productive and increase the ability to execute and learn motor tasks [29]. Non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) is one of the growing strategies in this regard and has been gaining attention over the last two decades [30, 31]. In particular, Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS), which is the most common NIBS technique, has shown promising results for improving cognitive and motor function in different target populations [32,33,34]. tDCS induces its effects by passing a direct current up to 4 mA through the skull using at least two electrodes (anode and cathode), which may modulate the neuronal excitability of different areas of the brain [35,36,37,38]. Although applying a smaller and more focal target electrode, and a larger reference electrode is gaining popularity in recent tDCS studies, the most frequently used electrode sizes are between 25 and 35 cm2 [39]. In this regard, M1 and DLPFC are the most common areas investigated in previous studies mostly because of their substantial role in regulating exercise and cognitive function [40,41,42,43]. Indeed, various studies have shown that stimulating either M1 or DLPFC improves various aspects of exercise performance and cognition [41, 44,45,46,47,48]. Interestingly, recent findings have shown that the activation of M1 and DLPFC areas can be effective in learning movement skills and its underlying neurophysiological mechanisms are related to the changes in neuronal excitability, neurotransmitters, synaptic plasticity, and brain network functional connections [49]. It has also been reported that anodal tDCS over the M1 area results in faster skill acquisition [50]. Accordingly, the anodal unihemispheric concurrent dual-site tDCS (a-tDCSUHCDS) was recently proposed as an effective strategy for simultaneously stimulating M1 and DLPFC, yielding a greater increase in corticospinal excitability compared to the isolated stimulation of M1 and DLPFC [51,52,53]. It is noteworthy that this increase in corticospinal excitability has been shown to last for 24 h after a-tDCSUHCDS [51]. Talimkhani et al. [54] provided further support for the effectiveness of a-tDCSUHCDS by showing its positive effect on the acquisition of cognitive skills and functions in healthy subjects.

Considering that both VR and tDCS may improve different facets of cognitive and motor function, one could speculate that adding a-tDCSUHCDS to VR training could provide a synergistic effect leading to more positive and long-lasting outcomes, particularly in a sensitive population like adolescent girls. To fill this gap, we aimed to investigate the effects of the concurrent application of a-tDCSUHCDS and VR on motor coordination in sedentary adolescent girls. We hypothesized that the addition of a-tDCSUHCDS to the VR protocol would result in an enhancement in motor coordination, as measured by EHC and BC, compared to the use of sham-tDCS with the VR protocol in sedentary adolescent girls.

Materials and methods

Study design

This study was a single-blinded and sham-controlled randomized controlled trial with three parallel groups (between-subject design), conducted within four weeks. The study protocol was pre-registered in the Iranian Registry of Clinical Trials (IRCT id: IRCT20221124056598N1) and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Razi University (IR.RAZI.REC.1401.058). All the experimental procedures were conducted following the declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from the parents of the subjects before participating in the study.

First, in a familiarization session, the subjects got acquainted with the whole experimental procedure, interventions, and measuring outcome variables. In this session, anthropometric characteristics such as height and weight were also measured. The height and weight of each subject were employed to calculate the body mass index (BMI) for subsequent comparisons. Then, the pre-test measurements including EHC and BC were conducted. Following the pre-test, the subjects were randomly assigned to one of the three experimental groups including (1) VR + a-tDCSUHCDS, (2) VR + sham-tDCS (s-tDCSUHCDS), and (3) Control, each containing 12 participants. The VR + a-tDCSUHCDS and VR + s-tDCSUHCDS took part in a four-week protocol including three sessions per week (12 sessions in total). In each session, the VR + a-tDCSUHCDS group first received 20 min of anodal tDCSUHCDS followed by the VR. The VR + s-tDCSUHCDS followed the same approach except that they received 20 min of sham stimulation before VR in each session. The participants in the control group were asked to maintain their usual routines (i.e., their usual activities of daily living) and did not receive any form of intervention. They only participated in the outcome measures assessment. The study variables were measured at baseline (pre-test), 24 h after the intervention phase (after 4 weeks), and 2 weeks after the termination of the intervention phase as the retention test.

Participants

Thirty-six sedentary adolescent girls participated in the present study. The sample size was calculated using G*Power software (Version 3.1.9.2, Kiel, Germany) as follows: test family = F tests; Statistical test = ANOVA: Repeated measures, within-between interaction; α error probability = 0.05; power (1-β err prob) = 0.80; Effect size f = 0.3, number of groups = 3, number of measurements = 3, Correlation among repeated measures = 0.5, Nonsphericity correction ɛ = 1. The effect size used for calculating the sample size was derived from a review article in which the effect of tDCS on motor learning and memory performance was evaluated [55]. Accordingly, 27 participants would be appropriate as the sample size for the present study. Considering the possibility of dropouts considering the number of sessions and their duration and the whole study procedure [56], 36 participants were recruited for this study. Interestingly, there were no dropouts, and all the participants from the initial pool completed the whole experimental procedure. Inclusion criteria were: (a) being a sedentary girl based on the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ), (b) aged 15–18 years, (c) being right-handed, and (d) having a normal or corrected vision. Also, the exclusion criteria were: (a) having a history of neurological disease, (b) having cardiovascular disease, (c) having any metal implants in the brain, (d) a history of disturbance in balance, recurrent postural dizziness, and fear of electrical stimulation of the brain, (e) failure to adhere to the study’s instructions, including participation, interventions, and measurements during testing, (f) any weakness or physical injury, and (g) inability to finish test and exercise sessions. The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 Average age, height, and Weight in three groups (Mean ± SD)

International physical activity questionnaire (IPAQ)

IPAQ was used to evaluate the level of PA. This questionnaire is a short version of IPAQ and has both acceptable validity and reliability [57, 58]. According to the scoring procedure of the IPAQ, participants who scored less than “600 MET minutes a week” were identified as sedentary (low level of physical activity) and participated in this study. Further detailed information regarding the scoring and analysis of the IPAQ can be found at www.ipaq.ki.se. IPAQ was used only for the participants’ screening process.

Randomization and allocation concealment

In this study, permuted block randomization was conducted via the www.randomization.comwebsite. This involved first allocating a unique number to each subject as an identification code, followed by the generation of a sequence of 36 digits (equivalent to the sample size). Treatment labels were then defined in the relevant section of the website, comprising: 1) VR + a-tDCSUHCDS, 2) VR + s-tDCSUHCDS, and 3) Control groups. Once the treatment groups had been defined, a permuted block randomization procedure was employed to mitigate potential issues associated with equal block sizes. In this instance, the block sizes were not equal and were instead multiples of the number of treatment groups (e.g., block sizes of 2, 4, 6, or 8). The website could randomly specify the sequence of blocks with different sizes. In the final step, the ‘Generate Plan’ function on the website was employed to randomly assign subjects to blocks of different sizes, which already had a random sequence. Finally, the group to which each subject belonged was determined by cross-referencing the subject number (identification code) with the blocks.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS)

tDCS was applied using two battery-driven stimulator devices with two channels each (NeuroStim 2, Medina Tebgostar, Tehran, Iran). Four carbon electrodes, two anodes (5 × 4 cm; 20 cm2; 0.10 mA/cm² each), and two cathodes (9 × 4; 36 cm2; 0.056 mA/cm²) electrodes, covered by saline-soaked surface sponges (NaCl 140 mmol dissolved in Milli-Q water) were used to concurrently stimulate target areas in the brain. The larger cathode electrodes were chosen to decrease the neuromodulatory effects of these electrodes. A 64-channel EEG cap with the international 10–20 EEG system positions was used to locate target areas over the scalp. The a-tDCSUHCDS montage was used for simultaneously targeting M1 and DLPFC areas [52, 54]. a-tDCSUHCDS was applied before VR training with 2 mA at each nominal target (i.e., 2 mA at M1 and 2 mA at DLPFC) for 20 min. One anode electrode was placed symmetrically over the Cz (2.5 cm on each side of the M1) targeting the motor cortex and the other anode electrode was placed vertically over F3 targeting the left DLPFC. The cathode electrodes were placed vertically over the supraorbital region, centered at AF4 and the other centered between Fpz and AFz. This montage was chosen based on recent studies showing that a-tDCSUHCDS is an effective strategy for simultaneous stimulation of M1 and DLPFC yielding higher and long-lasting corticospinal excitability compared to the isolated stimulation of M1 and DLPFC [51, 54]. In both a-tDCSUHCDS and sham tDCS conditions, the current was gradually ramped up and down at the beginning and end of stimulation for 30 s. In a-tDCSUHCDS, the current was maintained at 2 mA for 20 min. In the sham condition (s-tDCSUHCDS), the same electrode position was applied, but the 2-mA current was maintained only for 30 s. This protocol is adequate for blinding participants in tDCS studies [33, 59,60,61].

tDCS modeling

The computational modeling of the a-tDCSUHCDS-induced electric current in the brain was performed following a previous study [53]. The brain current flow during tDCS was calculated using a finite element model (FEM) following the standard pipeline in SimNIBS 4.0.0 [62]. The magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) head model template from the Montreal Neurologic Institute (MNI 152) available in the software was used for the simulation. MRI data were segmented into surfaces corresponding to the white matter (WM), gray matter (GM), cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), skull, and skin. The electrical conductivities of each segment were determined according to values previously established as follows: WM = 0.126 S/meter (S/m), GM = 0.275 S/m, CSF = 1.654 S/m, bone = 0.010 S/m, and skin/scalp = 0.465 S/m [63], rubber electrode = 29.4 S/m, and saline-soaked sponges = 1.000 S/m [63]. All information concerning the respective tDCS montages was entered into the software: current intensity = 2 mA for each anode; electrode position (+ F3 and + Cz / -Fpz and -AF4); electrode and sponge sizes (anodes 5 × 4 cm and cathodes 9 × 4 cm); electrode thickness = 1 mm; sponge thickness = 5 mm. The results of the simulations are presented in Fig. 1, in terms of the electric field strength (Fig. 1C, D, E, and F) and radial (Fig. 1G, H, I, and J) electric field (normal to the cortical surface), both of which are most important for neuromodulatory effects of tDCS [64]. As can be seen in Fig. 1C, D, E, and F, the study montage has reached our target areas with enough electric current strength to induce a neuromodulatory effect (> 0.2–0.25 V/m) [65]. Furthermore, the target areas were stimulated with the desired polarity (i.e., anodal current) to induce excitatory effects in the target regions (Fig. 1G, H, I, and J). Other areas such as the supplementary motor area, premotor cortex, and other prefrontal cortex were also stimulated in the current path from the anodal to the cathodal electrode. This spread electric current is a characteristic of tDCS applied with large rectangular pads (the so-called ‘conventional’ tDCS) [66]. The figure was reproduced from Banaei et al. [53] who used the same a-tDCSUHCDS as in the present study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Strength and radial component of the electric field induced by tDCS, reproduced from Banaei et al. [53]

Finite Element Models derived from Magnetic Resonance Imaging in a head model (MNI152) of the strength and radial (normal to the cortical surface) component of the electric field (EF) induced by tDCS. Electrode montage targeting the simultaneous stimulation with anodal tDCS of the representation of the lower limbs in the primary motor cortex and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (A and B), with red electrodes representing the anodes (5 × 4 cm) and blue electrodes representing the cathodes (9 × 4 cm). The EF strength is presented in the color-coded figures (C, D, E, and F), with hotter colors indicating stronger EF and colder colors indicating the opposite. The radial EF is presented in the color-coded figures (G, H, I, and J), where red color represents the electric current flowing into the cortex (i.e., inducing excitatory effects) and blue color represents the electric current flowing out of the cortex (i.e., inducing inhibitory effects). The study montage has reached the target areas with enough electric current strength to induce a neuromodulatory effect, as shown in figures E and F (blue circles roughly representing the target areas). Furthermore, the target areas were stimulated with the desired polarity (i.e., anodal current) to induce excitatory effects in the target regions, as shown in panels I and J (blue circles roughly representing the target areas)

Virtual reality

For VR we used Xbox Kinect, which is a commercial videogame console involving a combination of software and hardware (Xbox 360 and Kinect Xbox 360, Microsoft) connected to a television (47” LG). The hardware includes a system consisting of a projector, an infrared camera, and a special microchip. Kinect is a motion assessment device that no longer needs special clothing and sensor connectivity, and at the same time as the person moves, it transfers the position of the body to the device and the game, creating a connection between the real and virtual worlds [67]. The VR sessions were conducted three times per week, on non-consecutive days, over four weeks (12 sessions in total). In each session, the participants received 20 min of either a-tDCSUHCDS or sham, after which they proceeded to perform the VR session for 60 min. In this study, the Kinect sports games Skiing, Ping Pong, Boxing, and Golf were employed. Each game was played for 15 min, resulting in a total duration of 60 min for each VR intervention session. The order in which the four games were presented was not fixed; participants were permitted to select the order of the games according to their preferences. However, they were required to play all four games in each session. Each game was presented only once in a single session, and the VR intervention was conducted in a quiet, distraction-free environment.

Control group

The complete control group did not receive any intervention with tDCS (neither anodal nor sham) or VR. However, they underwent the same evaluation protocol at baseline (pre-test), after the intervention phase (after 4 weeks), and 2 weeks after the termination of the intervention phase as the retention test.

Eye-hand coordination (EHC) assessment

An Automatic Mirror Trace (Model 58024 A, Lafayette Instrument Company, Indiana, USA) device was used to measure the EHC. The validity of this device has already been proven [68, 69]. The device consisted of an aluminum plate with a nonconducting black star pattern anodized onto the surface, a metal plate, a mirror, and a metallic-tracing stylus. The aluminum plate and the metallic-tracing stylus were both connected to the Silent Impulse Counter (Model 58024 C, Lafayette Instrument Company, Indiana, USA). The metal plate was positioned above the surface in a way that the participants were just able to see their tracing hand through the mirror and not directly. The pertinent instructions were given to the participants to minimize errors and complete the task as fast as possible. The participants were then asked to position the stylus over the pattern and draw a star, both clockwise and counter-clockwise. A full clockwise and counter-clockwise drawing was considered one attempt. Each participant was asked to perform five attempts with their dominant hand, with one minute of rest between the attempts. During the task, touching the aluminum plate by the stylus (stylus outside of the star pattern) was defined as an error. The number of errors and the time to complete the task were used for calculating the score of each attempt using the following formula: “Standard Score for Each Attempt = (100 – number of errors) / (completion time in seconds)”. The mean value of these five attempts was calculated as the total score of each participant and used for statistical analysis. Higher scores represented better EHC. The EHC was assessed at baseline (pre-test), after the intervention phase (after 4 weeks), and 2 weeks after the termination of the intervention phase as the retention test.

Bimanual coordination (BC) test

A Two-Arm Coordination Test device (Model 32532, Lafayette Instrument Company, Indiana, USA) was used to measure the BC. The validity of this instrument has been previously demonstrated [70]. The device consisted of a Tracing Apparatus connected to the Economy Clock/Counter (Model 54060 A, Lafayette Instrument Company, Indiana, USA). Participants were asked to sit directly in front of the two-arm tracing apparatus. The test was composed of two operations including tracing a star in a clockwise and a counter-clockwise direction. The participants were instructed on how to manipulate the stylus by moving the handles of the apparatus; spreading the handles made the stylus move toward the top of the board; bringing the handles together moved the stylus downward on the board. Lateral movement was also accomplished by simultaneously moving both handles to the left or the right. It was then explained to the participants that the objective of the test was to manipulate the handles in such a way as to keep the stylus on the black star pattern and move it around the star as quickly as possible, making as few errors as possible. A full clockwise and counter-clockwise drawing was considered one attempt. Each participant was asked to perform five attempts, with one minute of rest in between. During the task, touching the aluminum plate by the stylus (stylus outside of the star pattern) was defined as an error. The number of errors and the time to complete the task were used for calculating the score of each attempt using the following formula: “Standard Score for Each Attempt = (100 – number of errors) / (completion time in seconds)”. The mean value of these five attempts was calculated as the total score of each participant and used for statistical analysis. Higher scores represented better BC. The BC was assessed at baseline (pre-test), after the intervention phase (after 4 weeks), and 2 weeks after the termination of the intervention phase as the retention test.

Statistical analysis

The statistical analyses were performed using SPSS software, version 23 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). The normal distribution of each data set was evaluated by the Shapiro-Wilk normality test. All data were normally distributed, and accordingly, a two-way mixed ANOVA test was employed for statistical analyses (a 3 × 3 factorial design, time as the within-subject factor with three levels, and group as the between-subject factor with three levels). When a significant interaction effect was observed, the Bonferroni-corrected post hoc test was applied for pairwise comparisons. In case of a violation in the assumption of sphericity, as assessed using the Mauchly test, the Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon correction was applied. Partial eta squared (η2p) was used as a measure of the effect size for the ANOVAs and interpreted as small (0.01–0.059), medium (0.06 to 0.139), or large (≥ 0.14). Cohen’s d (dav) calculation of the effect size was also used for pairwise comparison and interpreted as small (0.20–0.49), medium (0.50–0.79), or large (≥ 0.80). Moreover, the “percentage difference” between the groups at each time [(%Δ = 100 × \(\:\frac{|Mgroup1-Mgroup2}{\frac{Mgroup1+Mgroup2}{2}}\:),\:M:mean\)] [71] and the “percentage change” within each group over time [(%Δ = 100 × \(\:\frac{Mpost-Mpre\:}{Mpre}),\:M:mean\:\)] were calculated and reported as (%Δ). The significance level for all tests was defined as p<0.05. All values in the figures are presented as the mean ± standard deviation (M ± SD).

Results

The mean values of the EHC of each experimental group at three specified time points (baseline, post-intervention, and retention) are presented in Table 2. The results showed significant main effects of group (F(2,33)= 28.2, p< 0.001, η2p= 0.631) and time (F(2,66)= 153.07.3, p< 0.001, η2p= 0.823), and also “group × time” interaction (F(4,66)= 30.3, p< 0.001, η2p= 0.647) on the EHC. Pairwise comparisons revealed that EHC improved significantly from baseline to post-intervention (p< 0.001, dav= 4.3, Δ= +325.5% and p< 0.001, dav= 2.6, Δ= +208.5%) and retention test (p< 0.001, dav= 5.1, Δ= +360.4% and p< 0.001, dav= 2.8, Δ= +221.2%) in the VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS group, respectively, while no significant change was observed from the post-intervention to the retention test in both groups (p> 0.05). Pairwise comparisons also demonstrated that the EHC was significantly higher at post-intervention in the VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS groups than the control group (p< 0.001, dav= 3.1, Δ = 103.7%; p< 0.0001, dav= 2.4, Δ = 85.7%, respectively). Also, at the retention test, the EHC was significantly higher in the VR + a-tDCS compared to both the VR + s-tDCS (p = 0.024, dav = 1.04, Δ = 26.9%) and control (p< 0.001, dav= 3.8, Δ = 104.6%) groups. The EHC was also significantly higher in the VR + s-tDCS group compared to the control group (p< 0.001, dav= 2.2, Δ = 83.5%) at the retention, Fig2(A).

Table 2 Mean values of the Eye-Hand and Bimanual Coordination test score in three groups at Baseline, after the intervention and follow-up (Mean ± SD)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean values of the eye-hand coordination (A) and bimanual coordination test (B) in three experimental groups at the specified time points. VR + a-tDCS = virtual reality and anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation; VR + s-tDCS = virtual reality and sham Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation. * = significantly different from baseline withing the same group (all ps<0.001); # = significantly different from post-intervention withing the same group (p = 0.008); & = significantly different from control at the same timepoint (all ps<0.001); $ = significantly different from VR + s-tDCS at the same timepoint (all ps < 0.03)

The mean values of the BC of each experimental group at three specified time points (baseline, post-intervention, and retention) are presented in Table 2. The results demonstrated that there were significant main effects of group (F(2,33)= 37.5, p< 0.001, η2p=0.695) and time (F(2,66)= 113.7.3, p< 0.001, η2p= 0.775), and also “group × time” interaction (F(4,66)= 30.4, p< 0.001, η2p= 0.649) on the BC. Pairwise comparisons revealed that BC improved significantly from baseline to post-intervention (p< 0.001, dav= 3.4, Δ= +114.6% and p< 0.001, dav= 2.6, Δ= +112.1%) and to the retention test (p< 0.001, dav= 4.7, Δ= +168.5% and p< 0.001, dav= 2.1, Δ= +82.9%) in the VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS group, respectively. BC was also improved significantly from post-intervention to the retention test (p = 0.008, dav= 0.9, Δ= +25.1%) in the VR + a-tDCS but not in the VR + s-tDCS group (p> 0.05).BC was also significantly higher at post-intervention (p< 0.001, dav= 3.1, Δ = 79.8% and p< 0.0001, dav= 2.6, Δ = 71.8%) and retention (p< 0.001, dav= 4.2, Δ = 95.06%; p< 0.001, dav= 2.2, Δ = 55.3%) in the VR + a-tDCS and VR + s-tDCS groups, respectively, compared to the control group. The BC was also significantly higher in the VR + a-tDCS group compared to the VR + s-tDCS group (p< 0.001, dav= 2.3, Δ = 45.7%) at the retention; Fig. 2(B).

Discussion

This study assessed the effect of a-tDCSUHCDS targeting both M1 and left DLPFC regions in addition to VR sports games on motor coordination of sedentary adolescent girls. Our findings provided more support for the efficacy of VR sports games in improving coordination skills as the EHC and BC were significantly higher in VR + a-tDCSUHCDS and VR + sh-tDCS groups compared to the control group at post-intervention and retention test. Moreover, as a novel finding, we observed that adding the a-tDCSUHCDS to the VR protocol resulted in higher improvement in EHC and BC compared to the sham condition at retention.

Our results showed that the VR + a-tDCSUHCDS and VR + sh-tDCS groups outperformed the control group in both coordination tasks (EHC and BC). Hence, we could assume that VR games used in the present study were able to improve coordination skills (when controlling the effects of a-tDCSUHCDS) in sedentary adolescent girls, which is in line with the results of previous studies [20, 26, 27, 72,73,74]. Preliminary evidence supports this type of game as an enjoyable medium for low to moderate-intensity PA [75]. Providing accessible and attractive options for PA at home might overcome many of the reported barriers to PA, particularly for high-risk disability groups [76]. VR has been shown to increase motor coordination accuracy by improving visual feedback [18, 21]. These games can develop visual, auditory, and tactile inputs to improve coordination through repetition and practice, and provide feedback and motivation [19]. Given that such games involve controlling each hand separately, an improvement in coordination is not far-fetched [20]. In addition, due to the use of Kinect in the VR games, participants had to use their whole body to continue playing and interacting with the graphical environment, which could also be an effective factor in improving motor coordination in sedentary adolescent girls after VR intervention without anodal brain stimulation. In fact, given the distance between the participants and the screen, they should use visual-spatial skills, bimanual coordination, eye-hand coordination, and shorter reaction time to control and execute the game properly [77]. According to the observational learning theory, learning motor skills from observation occurs through an input-output cognitive model, in which attention, retention, motor reproduction, and motivation are four processes that account for learning from observation [78]. It seems that this model can somehow explain the underlying mechanisms by which VR games can positively affect coordination skills. During the VR games, the participants would receive visual feedback from the screen and regulate it via the attentional process. This information could be stored for memory representation via the retention process, and then be converted into actions that resemble the modeled behavior via the reproduction process [78,79,80]. Finally, whether or not these behaviors become overt actions is determined by motivational processes [78].

On the other hand, the present study showed that the VR + a-tDCSUHCDS group outperformed the VR + sh-tDCS group in EHC and BC at the retention test (2 weeks after the end of interventions). Previous studies have paved the way for investigating the potential synergistic effects between tDCS and other intervention strategies in healthy and clinical populations [53, 81, 82]. Our results are in line with previous studies showing positive effects of tDCS on motor function and coordination [13, 83,84,85]. The DLPFC at the top of the motor hierarchy and the M1 at the bottom play an important role in the learning and performance of motor skills [15, 31, 43]. Interestingly, neuroplastic changes from higher-order areas (i.e. DLPFC) to lower-order areas (i.e. M1) are essential for procedural motor learning [86]. It seems that the tDCS montage used in the present study was able to induce favorable changes in the DLPFC and M1 areas leading to improvement in coordination skills. This could be attributed to the enhanced spatial coordinate system and motor coordinate system, which correspond to the main function of the DLPFC and M1, respectively [86]. In addition, one of the novel aspects of the present study was that tDCS was applied in each session before the VR games were performed. It should be noted that most previous studies have used a single tDCS session, whereas in our study we applied 12 a-tDCSUHCDS sessions on the same days as the VR games. The application of repeated sessions of tDCS is thought to have a cumulative effect that promotes/regulates the efficiency of information processing in neural circuits, leading to the synthesis of several proteins and subsequently to long-term-like potentiation (LTP) [87]. In other words, increasing excitability during successive tDCS sessions increases the chances of stronger and more effective synaptic communication between neurons activated during motor skill learning.

According to the literature, when it comes to synergistic effects between two or more interventions, the key factor is whether or not these interventions activate common mechanistic pathways [82]. We believe that the tDCS montage [simultaneous stimulation of DLPFC and M1 (dual-site tDCS)] and its repetitive nature (applied at each session before the VR games) were able to induce synergistic effects with the VR games, leading to greater outcome measures (higher EHC and BC) compared to the sham condition. Neuroimaging and fMRI studies have shown the involvement of DLPFC and M1 areas during exposure to VR games [88, 89]. Therefore, it seems that the tDCS montage used in the present study had a so-called pre-conditioning effect on the subsequent neural activities in the target brain areas triggered by the VR games. Such activation of shared neural circuits is likely to have provoked synergistic effects between tDCS and VR, culminating in additional benefits as seen in the higher motor coordination in the VR + a-tDCSUHCDS group compared to the VR + s-tDCS group. Jo et al. [90] provided further support for this claim by suggesting that the application of tDCS before training may be optimal for improving motor skill learning.

On the other hand, the results of the present study were not consistent with other previous studies that showed no effect of tDCS on motor skills [91,92,93]. The number of tDCS sessions may be one of the explanations for the divergent outcomes, because other studies used just one [91, 93] or four [92] tDCS sessions, while in this investigation 12 stimulation sessions were used. Therefore, the number of sessions may be one of the most important reasons for the efficacy of tDCS. Other reasons could be due to the duration of stimulation, since they used tDCS for 15 min [91, 93], while we applied tDCS for 20 min. In addition, the tDCS in this study was delivered offline (i.e., not during the VR exercises). In other studies, however, the stimulation was delivered online and at the same time as the exercises. Other possible reasons for the inconsistency between our results and previous studies could be related to stimulation intensity and electrode placement. In the studies by Furuya et al. [91] and McCambridge et al. [93], the stimulation intensity was 1 mA and the anode electrode was placed over the C3 or C4 areas, whereas in the present study, we used an intensity of 2 mA at each site to targeting to stimulate the DLPFC and M1 areas simultaneously.

Although all the necessary details were taken into account to ensure optimal control of the study procedure, the results of the present study must be interpreted with caution, as they are not free from the effects of limiting factors. Only inactive adolescent girls were included in this study, which limits the generalizability of the results to other populations. In addition, we were not able to include measures of brain activity (e.g., EEG, fNIRS, fMRI), corticocortical, corticospinal, and/or motor neuronal excitability, which would provide some direct insights into the possible mechanisms involved in the effects of tDCS and VR. Researchers should take these limitations into account when designing future studies. On the other hand, the strength of the present study includes the fact that we used repeated sessions of tDCS, whereas most of the tDCS literature is based on the effects of a single session. Furthermore, the fact that we used a novel dual-site tDCS montage in the motor learning/control area, targeting both M1 and the left DLPFC, could be considered a novel contribution of the present study, as these brain areas are important for motor learning/control. Finally, the combination of tDCS and VR training to assess possible synergistic effects could also be considered a novel contribution of the present study.

Conclusion

The present results suggest that the application of the uni-hemispheric concurrent dual-site anodal tDCS protocol simultaneously targeting M1 and left DLPFC over 12 sessions can increase the effectiveness of VR training and improve performance in motor coordination tasks, namely eye-hand and bimanual coordination in sedentary adolescent girls. Therefore, from a practical point of view, this intervention protocol (VR + a-tDCSUHCDS) could be considered effective in providing additional improvement in coordination skills. Our results may have practical implications for other healthy and clinical populations, so future studies are suggested to measure the efficacy of this intervention protocol in other target populations. Also, investigating the effectiveness of this intervention protocol on other motor functions could be another direction for future studies.

Data availability

No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

  1. Corder K, Sharp SJ, Atkin AJ, Griffin SJ, Jones AP, Ekelund U, et al. Change in objectively measured physical activity during the transition to adolescence. Br J Sports Med. 2015;49(11):730–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Yarcheski A, Mahon NE, Yarcheski TJ. Anger in early adolescent boys and girls with health manifestations. Nurs Res. 2002;51(4):229–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Raman A, Fitch MD, Hudes ML, Lustig RH, Murray CB, Ikeda JP, et al. Baseline correlates of insulin resistance in inner city high-BMI African‐American children. Obesity. 2008;16(9):2039–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Chaput J-P, Willumsen J, Bull F, Chou R, Ekelund U, Firth J, et al. 2020 WHO guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour for children and adolescents aged 5–17 years: summary of the evidence. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. 2020;17(1):141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Global trends in insufficient physical activity among adolescents: a pooled analysis of 298 population-based surveys with 1·6 million participants. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. 2020;4(1):23–35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Marques A, Henriques-Neto D, Peralta M, Martins J, Demetriou Y, Schönbach DMI et al. Prevalence of physical activity among adolescents from 105 low, Middle, and high-income countries. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(9).

  7. Bhukar J. Effect of six week coordinative drills on eye hand coordination of young Athletes. 2023.

  8. Vandorpe B, Vandendriessche J, Vaeyens R, Pion J, Matthys S, Lefevre J, et al. Relationship between sports participation and the level of motor coordination in childhood: a longitudinal approach. J Sci Med Sport. 2012;15(3):220–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Crawford JD, Medendorp WP, Marotta JJ. Spatial transformations for eye–hand coordination. J Neurophysiol. 2004.

  10. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Wu S, Nouredanesh M, Tung J, Christian LW. Development of eye-hand coordination in typically developing children and adolescents assessed using a reach-to-grasp sequencing task. Hum Mov Sci. 2021;80:102868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Telford RD, Cunningham RB, Telford RM, Olive LS, Byrne D, Abhayaratna W. Benefits of early development of eye–hand coordination: evidence from the LOOK longitudinal study. Scand J Med Sci Sports. 2013;23(5):e263–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Shetty AK, Shankar MSV, Annamalai N. Bimanual coordination: influence of age and gender. J Clin Diagn Research: JCDR. 2014;8(2):15–6.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Pixa NH, Pollok B. Effects of tDCS on Bimanual Motor skills: a brief review. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:63.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Rudisch J, Müller K, Kutz DF, Brich L, Sleimen-Malkoun R, Voelcker-Rehage C. How age, cognitive function and gender affect Bimanual Force Control. Front Physiol. 2020;11:245.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Yamamoto S, Ishii D, Ishibashi K, Kohno Y. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation of the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex modulates cognitive function related to motor execution during Sequential Task: a Randomized Control Study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2022;16:890963.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Srinivasan D, Martin BJ. Eye–hand coordination of symmetric bimanual reaching tasks: temporal aspects. Exp Brain Res. 2010;203(2):391–405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hamad A, Jia B. How virtual reality technology has changed our lives: an overview of the current and potential applications and limitations. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(18).

  18. Qian J, McDonough DJ, Gao Z. The Effectiveness of Virtual Reality Exercise on Individual’s Physiological, Psychological and Rehabilitative Outcomes: A Systematic Review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(11).

  19. Norouzi E, Gerber M, Pühse U, Vaezmosavi M, Brand S. Combined virtual reality and physical training improved the bimanual coordination of women with multiple sclerosis. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 2021;31(4):552–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sabel M, Sjolund A, Broeren J, Arvidsson D, Saury JM, Blomgren K, et al. Active video gaming improves body coordination in survivors of childhood brain tumours. Disabil Rehabil. 2016;38(21):2073–84.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Rutkowski S, Jakóbczyk A, Abrahamek K, Nowakowska A, Nowak M, Liska D, et al. Training using a commercial immersive virtual reality system on hand–eye coordination and reaction time in students: a randomized controlled trial. Virtual Reality. 2024;28(1):7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ren Z, Wu J. The effect of virtual reality games on the Gross Motor skills of children with cerebral palsy: a Meta-analysis of Randomized controlled trials. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16(20).

  23. Farič N, Smith L, Hon A, Potts HW, Newby K, Steptoe A, et al. A virtual reality exergame to engage adolescents in physical activity: mixed methods study describing the formative intervention development process. J Med Internet Res. 2021;23(2):e18161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Gao Z, Chen S, Pasco D, Pope Z. A meta-analysis of active video games on health outcomes among children and adolescents. Obes Rev. 2015;16(9):783–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Foley L, Maddison R. Use of active video games to increase physical activity in children: a (virtual) reality? Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2010;22(1):7–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Ma AWW, Qu L. The effect of exergaming on eye-hand coordination among primary school children: a pilot study. Adv Phys Educ. 2016;6(2):99–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Caro K, Tentori M, Martinez-Garcia AI, Alvelais M. Using the FroggyBobby exergame to support eye-body coordination development of children with severe autism. Int J Hum Comput Stud. 2017;105:12–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Schaerz S, Boyes M, Mohamed A. The Effect of Extended reality Exercise on Physical Activity and Physical Performance in Children and Youth: a scoping review. J Electron Gaming Esports. 2023;1(1):jege2023–0016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Kruger PE, Campher J, Smit C. The role of visual skills and its impact on skill performance of cricket players and sport science. Afr J Phys Health Educ Recreation Dance. 2009;15(4):605–23.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Ruff CC. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(2):174–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Teymoori H, Amiri E, Tahmasebi W, Hoseini R, Grospretre S, Machado D. Effect of tDCS targeting the M1 or left DLPFC on physical performance, psychophysiological responses, and cognitive function in repeated all-out cycling: a randomized controlled trial. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2023;20(1):97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Moslemi B, Azmodeh M, Tabatabaei M, Alivandi Vafa M. The Effect of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex: a review of its role on cognitive functions. Neurosci J Shefaye Khatam. 2019;8(1):129–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Etemadi M, Amiri E, Tadibi V, Grospretre S, Valipour Dehnou V, Machado D. Anodal tDCS over the left DLPFC but not M1 increases muscle activity and improves psychophysiological responses, cognitive function, and endurance performance in normobaric hypoxia: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Neurosci. 2023;24(1):25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Ghayebzadeh S, Zardoshtian S, Amiri E, Giboin LS, Machado D. Anodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex boosts decision making and functional impulsivity in female sports referees. Life (Basel). 2023;13(5).

  35. Rajji TK, Rogasch NC, Daskalakis ZJ, Fitzgerald PB. Neuroplasticity-based brain stimulation interventions in the study and treatment of schizophrenia: a review. Can J Psychiatry. 2013;58(2):93–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Guleyupoglu B, Schestatsky P, Edwards D, Fregni F, Bikson M. Classification of methods in transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) and evolving strategy from historical approaches to contemporary innovations. J Neurosci Methods. 2013;219(2):297–311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Brunoni AR, Nitsche MA, Bolognini N, Bikson M, Wagner T, Merabet L, et al. Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): challenges and future directions. Brain Stimul. 2012;5(3):175–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Nitsche MA, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Priori A, Lang N, Antal A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008;1(3):206–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Thair H, Holloway AL, Newport R, Smith AD. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS): a beginner’s guide for design and implementation. Front Neurosci. 2017;11:641.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Lattari E, Campos C, Lamego MK, Legey S, Neto GM, Rocha NB, et al. Can transcranial direct current stimulation improve muscle power in individuals with advanced weight-training experience? J Strength Conditioning Res. 2020;34(1):97–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Machado D, Unal G, Andrade SM, Moreira A, Altimari LR, Brunoni AR, et al. Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on exercise performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Stimul. 2019;12(3):593–605.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Machado D, Amiri E. Letter to the editor regarding single-session anodal transcranial direct current stimulation to enhance sport-specific performance in athletes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Stimul. 2023;16(5):1549–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Machado D, Amiri E. Critical considerations on tDCS-induced changes in corticospinal excitability and exercise performance: should we go beyond M1? J Physiol. 2023;601(23):5453–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Vitor-Costa M, Okuno NM, Bortolotti H, Bertollo M, Boggio PS, Fregni F, et al. Improving Cycling performance: Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation increases time to exhaustion in Cycling. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(12):e0144916.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Grospretre S, Grandperrin Y, Nicolier M, Gimenez P, Vidal C, Tio G, et al. Effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the psychomotor, cognitive, and motor performances of power athletes. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):9731.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Alix-Fages C, Romero-Arenas S, Castro-Alonso M, Colomer-Poveda D, Rio-Rodriguez D, Jerez-Martinez A et al. Short-term effects of Anodal Transcranial Direct current stimulation on endurance and maximal force production. A systematic review and Meta-analysis. J Clin Med. 2019;8(4).

  47. Angius L, Pageaux B, Hopker J, Marcora SM, Mauger AR. Transcranial direct current stimulation improves isometric time to exhaustion of the knee extensors. Neuroscience. 2016;339:363–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Angius L, Santarnecchi E, Pascual-Leone A, Marcora SM. Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation over the Left Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex improves inhibitory control and endurance performance in healthy individuals. Neuroscience. 2019;419:34–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Qi S, Liang Z, Wei Z, Liu Y, Wang X. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor skills learning in healthy adults through the activation of different brain regions: a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci. 2022;16.

  50. Kunaratnam N, Saumer TM, Kuan G, Holmes Z, Swarbrick D, Kiss A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation leads to faster acquisition of motor skills, but effects are not maintained at retention. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(9):e0269851.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Vaseghi B, Zoghi M, Jaberzadeh S. The effects of anodal-tDCS on corticospinal excitability enhancement and its after-effects: conventional vs. unihemispheric concurrent dual-site stimulation. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9:533.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Gurdiel-Alvarez F, Gonzalez-Zamorano Y, Lerma Lara S, Gomez-Soriano J, Taylor J, Romero JP et al. Effectiveness of Unihemispheric Concurrent Dual-Site Stimulation over M1 and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Stimulation on Pain Processing: a Triple Blind Cross-over Control Trial. Brain Sci. 2021;11(2).

  53. Banaei P, Tadibi V, Amiri E, Machado D. Concomitant dual-site tDCS and dark chocolate improve cognitive and endurance performance following cognitive effort under hypoxia: a randomized controlled trial. Sci Rep. 2023;13(1):16473.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Talimkhani A, Abdollahi I, Mohseni-Bandpei MA, Ehsani F, Khalili S, Jaberzadeh S. Differential effects of Unihemispheric Concurrent Dual-Site and Conventional tDCS on Motor Learning: a Randomized, Sham-controlled study. Basic Clin Neurosci. 2019;10(1):59–72.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Buch ER, Santarnecchi E, Antal A, Born J, Celnik PA, Classen J, et al. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: a consensus and critical position paper. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128(4):589–603.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bell ML, Kenward MG, Fairclough DL, Horton NJ. Differential dropout and bias in randomised controlled trials: when it matters and when it may not. BMJ. 2013;346:e8668.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. BashiriMoosavi F, Farmanbar R, Taghdisi M, AtrkarRoshan Z. Level of physical activity among girl high school students in Tarom County and relevant factors. Iran J Health Educ Health Promotion. 2015;3(2):133–40.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Amiri E, Gharakhanlou R, Rajabi H, Giboin LS, Rezasoltani Z, Azma K. Non-local muscle fatigue is mediated at spinal and supraspinal levels. Exp Brain Res. 2022;240(6):1887–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Gandiga PC, Hummel FC, Cohen LG. Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117(4):845–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Moreira A, Machado DGS, Moscaleski L, Bikson M, Unal G, Bradley PS, et al. Effect of tDCS on well-being and autonomic function in professional male players after official soccer matches. Physiol Behav. 2021;233:113351.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Moreira A, Machado D, Bikson M, Unal G, Bradley PS, Moscaleski L, et al. Effect of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Professional Female Soccer players’ recovery following official matches. Percept Mot Skills. 2021;128(4):1504–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Thielscher A, Antunes A, Saturnino GB. Field modeling for transcranial magnetic stimulation: a useful tool to understand the physiological effects of TMS? Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2015;2015:222–5.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Opitz A, Paulus W, Will S, Antunes A, Thielscher A. Determinants of the electric field during transcranial direct current stimulation. NeuroImage. 2015;109:140–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Rahman A, Reato D, Arlotti M, Gasca F, Datta A, Parra LC, et al. Cellular effects of acute direct current stimulation: somatic and synaptic terminal effects. J Physiol. 2013;591(10):2563–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Kronberg G, Bridi M, Abel T, Bikson M, Parra LC. Direct Current Stimulation modulates LTP and LTD: activity dependence and dendritic effects. Brain Stimul. 2017;10(1):51–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. da Silva Machado DG, Bikson M, Datta A, Caparelli-Daquer E, Unal G, Baptista AF, et al. Acute effect of high-definition and conventional tDCS on exercise performance and psychophysiological responses in endurance athletes: a randomized controlled trial. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):13911.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Saenz-de-Urturi Z, Garcia-Zapirain Soto B. Kinect-based virtual game for the elderly that detects incorrect body postures in real time. Sensors. 2016;16(5):704.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Homma S. Correlations between anxiety and the stress responses of electrogastrography (EGG) induced by the mirror drawing test (MDT). J Smooth Muscle Res. 2014;50:1–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Julius MS, Adi-Japha E. A developmental perspective in learning the mirror-drawing task. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Pennathur A, Contreras LR, Arcaute K, Dowling W. Manual dexterity of older Mexican American adults: a cross-sectional pilot experimental investigation. Int J Ind Ergon. 2003;32(6):419–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Cole TJ, Altman DG. Statistics notes: what is a percentage difference? BMJ. 2017;358:j3663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Ren K, Gong X-M, Zhang R, Chen X-H. Effects of virtual reality training on limb movement in children with spastic diplegia cerebral palsy. Zhongguo Dang Dai Er Ke Za Zhi = Chinese. J Contemp Pediatr. 2016;18(10):975–9.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Sharan D, Ajeesh P, Rameshkumar R, Mathankumar M, Paulina RJ, Manjula M. Virtual reality based therapy for post operative rehabilitation of children with cerebral palsy. Work. 2012;41(Supplement 1):3612–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Elsaeh M, Pudlo P, Djemai M, Bouri M, Thevenon A, Heymann I, editors. The effects of haptic-virtual reality game therapy on brain-motor coordination for children with hemiplegia: A pilot study. 2017 International Conference on Virtual Rehabilitation (ICVR); 2017: IEEE.

  75. Allcott-Watson H, Chater A, Troop N, Howlett N. A systematic review of interventions targeting physical activity and/or healthy eating behaviours in adolescents: practice and training. Health Psychol Rev. 2023:1–24.

  76. Biddiss E, Irwin J. Active video games to promote physical activity in children and youth: a systematic review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010;164(7):664–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Staiano AE, Calvert SL. Exergames for physical education courses: physical, social, and cognitive benefits. Child Dev Perspect. 2011;5(2):93–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Fryling MJ, Johnston C, Hayes LJ. Understanding observational learning: an interbehavioral approach. Anal Verbal Behav. 2011;27(1):191–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol Rev. 1977;84(2):191.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Han Y, Syed Ali SKB, Ji L. Use of Observational Learning to Promote Motor Skill Learning in Physical Education: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(16).

  81. Ji Y, Ni X, Zheng K, Jiang Y, Ren C, Zhu H, et al. Synergistic effects of aerobic exercise and transcranial direct current stimulation on executive function and biomarkers in healthy young adults. Brain Res Bull. 2023;202:110747.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Steinberg F, Pixa NH, Fregni F. A review of Acute Aerobic Exercise and Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation effects on cognitive functions and their potential synergies. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018;12:534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Pixa NH, Steinberg F, Doppelmayr M. High-definition transcranial direct current stimulation to both primary motor cortices improves unimanual and bimanual dexterity. Neurosci Lett. 2017;643:84–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Ciechanski P, Kirton A. Transcranial direct-current stimulation can enhance motor learning in children. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(5):2758–67.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Carter MJ, Maslovat D, Carlsen AN. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation applied over the supplementary motor area delays spontaneous antiphase-to-in-phase transitions. J Neurophysiol. 2015;113(3):780–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Cao N, Pi Y, Qiu F, Wang Y, Xia X, Liu Y, et al. Plasticity changes in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex associated with procedural sequence learning are hemisphere-specific. NeuroImage. 2022;259:119406.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Agboada D, Mosayebi-Samani M, Kuo MF, Nitsche MA. Induction of long-term potentiation-like plasticity in the primary motor cortex with repeated anodal transcranial direct current stimulation - better effects with intensified protocols? Brain Stimul. 2020;13(4):987–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Lin Y, Pi Y, Wang Y, Xia X, Qiu F, Cao N, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex mediates working memory processes in motor skill learning. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2022;59:102129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Jancke L, Cheetham M, Baumgartner T. Virtual reality and the role of the prefrontal cortex in adults and children. Front Neurosci. 2009;3(1):52–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  90. Jo NG, Kim GW, Won YH, Park SH, Seo JH, Ko MH. Timing-dependent effects of Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on Hand Motor function in healthy individuals: a randomized controlled study. Brain Sci. 2021;11(10).

  91. Furuya S, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Altenmüller E. Early optimization in finger dexterity of skilled pianists: implication of transcranial stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2013;14:1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Vancleef K, Meesen R, Swinnen SP, Fujiyama H. tDCS over left M1 or DLPFC does not improve learning of a bimanual coordination task. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):35739.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. McCambridge AB, Stinear JW, Byblow WD. Neurophysiological and behavioural effects of dual-hemisphere transcranial direct current stimulation on the proximal upper limb. Exp Brain Res. 2016;234:1419–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the participants for their commitment to the study. We would also like to express our sincere gratitude to the university officials and those who helped with data collection.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NSH, AH, EA, and DM conceptualized and designed the study. NSH, AH, and EA conducted the experiments. NSH, AH, EA, and DM participated in the formal analysis. NSH wrote the original draft of the manuscript. AH, EA, and DM reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ali Heirani.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval and consent to participate This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Razi University (IR.RAZI.REC.1401.058) and registered in the Iranian Registry of Clinical Trials (IRCT id: IRCT20221124056598N1; Registration Date: 10-12-2022). All the experimental procedures were conducted following the declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects before participation in the study.

Consent for publication

Not Applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shahbazi, N., Heirani, A., Amiri, E. et al. Effects of repeated unihemispheric concurrent dual-site tDCS and virtual reality games on motor coordination of sedentary adolescent girls. Behav Brain Funct 20, 20 (2024). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12993-024-00247-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12993-024-00247-w

Keywords