Skip to main content

The role of cerebellar-cortical connectivity in modulating attentional abilities: insight from football athletes

Abstract

Neuroplasticity, a phenomenon present throughout the lifespan, is thought to be influenced by physical training. However, the relationship between neuroplastic differences and attentional abilities remains unclear. This study explored the differences in brain function and attentional abilities between professional football athletes and novices, and further investigated the relationship between the two. To address this question, we included 49 football athletes and 63 novices in our study, collecting data on resting-state functional connectivity and Attention Network Test (ANT). Behavioral results from the ANT indicated that football experts had superior orienting attention but weaker alerting functions compared to novices, with no difference in executive control attention. fMRI results revealed that football experts exhibited higher fractional Amplitude of Low-Frequency Fluctuations (fALFF) values in the bilateral anterior cerebellar lobes, bilateral insula, and left superior temporal gyrus. Functional connectivity analysis showed increased connectivity between the left anterior cerebellar lobe and various cortical regions, including the right supramarginal gyrus, left precuneus, left superior frontal gyrus, bilateral posterior cerebellar lobes, and bilateral precentral gyri in experts compared to novices. More importantly, in the expert group but not in novice group, functional connectivity differences significantly predicted attentional orienting scores. Graph theoretical analysis showed that experts exhibited higher betweenness centrality and node efficiency in the right cerebellar lobule III (Cerebelum_3_R) node. Our findings demonstrate that long-term professional football training may significantly affect neuroplasticity and attentional functions. Importantly, our analysis reveals a substantive connection between these two aspects, suggesting that the integration of neuroplastic and attentional changes is likely mediated by cerebellar-cortical connectivity.

Introduction

Neuroplasticity is widely recognized as a phenomenon that occurs throughout the lifespan. Professional athletes represent an ideal group for investigating the neuroplastic changes prompted by sports training. Employing the expert-novice paradigm, notable distinctions in brain structure and function have been identified between elite athletes and non-athletes [40, 46, 89,90,91, 95, 96, 98]. However, few studies have explored the relationship between neuroplastic differences and attentional abilities in athletes versus non-athletes.

Previous studies demonstrated that motor training can induce both functional and structural plasticity in the brain, with these neural plasticity changes involving extensive areas across the brain [4, 11, 17, 28, 56, 73, 89, 94]. A recent meta-analysis suggested that extensive motor training led to potential plastic changes in athletes’ brains [34]. Moreover, a systematic review by Hortobágyi et al. [29] indicated that exercise intensity was positively associated with neuroplasticity in healthy young adults, and that exercise-induced neuroplasticity gains were linked to motor changes. Similarly, enhanced functional connectivity (FC) was identified between the posterior cerebellar lobe and the fusiform gyrus in elite athletes [98]. Vahdat et al. [79] found changes in a network comprising cerebellar cortex, primary motor cortex, and dorsal premotor cortex that were linked to the motor aspects of learning. Moreover, Yan et al. [89] demonstrated alterations in functional connectivity and topological efficiency of the brain functional network among the motor cortex, visual cortex and cerebellum were significantly correlated with training intensity parameters. In summary, these studies indicate that motor training has a consistent impact on the neuroplasticity of the motor cortex and the cerebellum.

In the present study, we employed resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (rs-fMRI) to investigate the plasticity changes of brain function. Compared to task-based fMRI, rs-fMRI allows for the observation of intrinsic and spontaneous Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) activity in the brain without the potential interference from cognitive strategies [41]. Moreover, rs-fMRI patterns remain relatively stable over time, making them valuable for studying individual differences and the effects of long-term training on brain networks [27]. We calculated fractional Amplitude of Low-Frequency Fluctuations (fALFF), FC, and graph theory metrics to investigate the impact of football training on brain functional plasticity. The fALFF quantifies the low-frequency oscillatory power of local neuronal populations, reflecting spontaneous neural activity [100]. Compared to the older ALFF metric, fALFF is less susceptible to physiological noise [101] and has been associated with experience-dependent plasticity, as repeated training or skill acquisition can reshape intrinsic brain function [95, 96, 98]. Therefore, fALFF serves as an important measure of neural plasticity. Functional connectivity is defined as the temporal correlation between spatially remote neurophysiological events [25]. It can capture the organization of brain networks and the interactions between specific regions or networks. Moreover, FC has demonstrated stability within a single fMRI session and reliability over longer intervals (hours, weeks, or months), suggesting it may serve as a potential biomarker of training- or experience-driven neural plasticity [12], [17]. However, approaches focusing solely on regional brain activity or FC analysis fall short in fully encapsulating the complexity of brain functional connections and lack a description of the plasticity of the global brain functional network organization [80]. Thus, we conducted a further graph theory analysis to evaluate the differences in brain network organization between football athletes and novices. Graph theory-based approaches model the brain as a complex network of nodes and edges, offering a valuable means of examining neuroplasticity at the network level. In the virtual graph, nodes indicate anatomical elements (e.g., brain regions), and edges represent the relationships between nodes (e.g., connectivity) [85]. We calculated both nodal network metrics including betweenness centrality (BC), degree centrality (DC), node efficiency and small world parameters including normalized clustering coefficient (λ), small-worldness (σ) and normalized characteristic path length (γ). Nodal network metrics quantifies how important a node is within a network. The degree of a node in a network is defined as the count of edges that are directly connected to that node. Betweenness centrality is a measure that quantifies the extent to which a node controls the information flow between different pairs of nodes within the network. It reflects the node’s potential to exert influence over the communication pathways among the network’s constituents. Nodal efficiency is a measure of how well a node can communicate with others in a network. It indicates the node's contribution to information propagation. A node with high centrality is often seen as a hub in the brain network [9, 68]. The small-world model is a compelling framework for characterizing intricate brain networks, as it facilitates a dual approach to information processing: it allows for both specialized and modular operations as well as integrated and distributed functions. Furthermore, this model optimizes the efficiency of information exchange while maintaining a relatively low cost in terms of network connectivity [67].

There remains ongoing debate regarding whether and how long-term sports training induces changes in attention. Some studies indicated that elite athletes possessed superior attentional functions compared to non-athletes. For example, one previous study indicated elite athletes that participation in strategic sports, such as football, was associated with improved attention performance [60]. Similarly, Meng et al. [49] found that volleyball players exhibited enhanced attentional alerting compared to healthy controls. Moreover, a meta-analytic review found the athletes performed better on measures of a category of varied attentional paradigms [82]. Furthermore. Hüttermann et al. [31] found 25% greater attention breadth in expert athletes than in novices and the distribution of focused attention for experts varied as a function of the type of athletic expertise. However, some studies found that athletes did not exhibit improved attentional functions compared to the general population [48], and the prevalence of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) was even higher among athletes than in the general population [58]. Therefore, more research, especially studies with larger sample sizes, is needed to explore the impact of motor training on attentional functions.

Attention refers to the selective processing of information, prioritizing certain inputs over others that compete for the brain's limited cognitive resources [21]. Posner and Petersen, [57] proposed that the sources of attention constitute a specific system of anatomical areas, which can be further divided into three distinct networks. These networks are responsible for the functions of alerting, orienting, and executive control. Alerting was defined as the establishment of an enhanced state of vigilance in preparation for an imminent stimulus. Orienting was described as the ability to direct attentional resources towards a particular stimulus or spatial location, selecting this information for further processing. Executive control was deemed necessary to manage situations where the available information triggered inappropriate behavioral responses conflicting with current intentions [44]. These three functions are considered to be independent of each other [22]. The Attentional Network Test (ANT [22]) is an assessment tool designed to measure the efficiency of these three functions at a behavioral level in the same individual simultaneously. Previous study suggested engagement in strategic sports (football) was associated with enhanced performance on ANT [60]. Lu et al. [40] demonstrated that elite shooting and archery athletes on the national team are more efficient in all three attention networks. These studies indicate that professional athletic training is associated with changes in attentional functions.

An important aspect of neuroplasticity research is exploring the relationship between neural plasticity changes and cognitive abilities [50]. However, few studies directly explored the relationship between neuroplastic differences and cognitive abilities in professional athletes compared to non-athletes. In present study, we investigated the differences in brain function and attentional functions between football athletes and novices, and further explored the potential relationship between them. Based on consistent findings from previous research, we hypothesized that significant differences existed between football athletes and novices in brain regions such as the cerebellum, motor cortex, and the superior temporal gyrus (STG). Furthermore, we hypothesized that football athletes had superior attentional functions compared to novices. More importantly, we predicted that changes in brain functional plasticity were associated with alterations in attentional functions.

Materials and methods

Participants

We recruited a total of 67 active professional football athletes from the Shanghai Greenland Shenhua Football Club and 86 students without football experience from Shanghai University of Sport. All football athletes hold a Certificate of National First-Level Athlete of China and have undergone more than 5 years of professional football training (mean = 9.724 years, SD = 2.729, range = 3–16 years). Thirty participants did not complete the fMRI scan due to having personal preference, metal implants in the body or other physical reasons. Eleven participants were excluded from the analysis due to excessive head movement (Excluding Criteria: 2.0 mm and 2.0 degree in max head motion). Therefore, the current study included 49 football athletes (28 females) and 63 football novices (36 females). The football novices had no prior experience in football training, and did not have the habit of watching football matches. We matched the two groups for age(experts: M ± SD = 20.408 ± 1.707, range:18–25; novices: M ± SD = 19.905 ± 1.583, range:17–24, t = 1.613, p = 0.110, Cohen’s d = 0.307) and level of education (expert: 6 graduate students, 43 undergraduate students; novices: 8 graduate students, 55 undergraduate students). All participants were right-handed, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and reported no history of neurological disorders. All experimental procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of Shanghai University of Sport, China. Informed consent was obtained in written form from all participants prior to their involvement in the study.

Behavioral task

Prior to the MRI scan, participants were required to complete the Attention Network Test (For the task procedure, see Fig. 1.). The ANT is a psychological testing tool used to assess the functionality of an individual’s attention networks [22]. Subjects viewed five horizontal lines on a 15.6-inch (39.6 cm) computer screen, each positioned above or below a central fixation cross. The middle line contained arrowheads pointing left or right. Subjects were tasked with rapidly and accurately pressing a keyboard button to indicate the direction of the central arrowhead. To evaluate the three attentional functions, three conditions were used: (1) An asterisk appeared before the target line, measuring alerting effectiveness by the decrease in response time. (2) The asterisk was shown at the target arrow’s location, with improved performance indicating the orienting network's function. (3) The target line was flanked by lines with arrowheads pointing in the same (congruent) or opposite (incongruent) direction as the target, with the reaction time variance measuring executive control efficacy. Each participant completed 207 trials, including 15 practice trials with feedback. The entire session, with breaks, lasted approximately 20 min.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Description of the Attention Network Test. Following a 1000 ms (ms) fixation point, cues (four types of cues) are presented, and after a 400 ms interval, participants need to determine the direction of the central arrow

Functional imaging

Imaging data were acquired using a Siemens Prisma 3 T scanner with a 64-channel phase-array head coil at the Imaging Center for MRI Research, Shanghai University of Sport. Participants were positioned supine with their heads stabilized using foam pads to reduce movement during scanning. The task-based functional imaging data included 252 continuous whole-brain functional volumes (scan duration: 8.4 min). These were captured using a gradient echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms; echo time (TE) = 30 ms; 58 slices; voxel size = 2 × 2 × 2.4 mm3; flip angle = 90°. Additionally, a high-resolution T1-weighted anatomical MRI was obtained for each participant with the following specifications: TR = 2530 ms; TE = 2.98 ms; 192 slices; voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3; flip angle = 7°.

Data analysis

Behavioral analysis

Following Fan et al. [22], the calculation of mean RT in this study excluded error trials. The alerting effect was measured by subtracting the mean RT in the double-cue conditions from the mean RT in the no-cue conditions for each group (alerting effect: RTno-cue − RTdouble-cue). The orienting effect was calculated by subtracting the mean RT in the spatial cue conditions from the mean RT in the center cue conditions for each group (orienting effect: RTcenter-cue − RTspatial-cue). The conflict effect was evaluated by subtracting the mean RT in the congruent flanking conditions from the mean RT in the incongruent flanking conditions for each group (conflict effect: RTincongruent − RTcongruent ​). The above calculation methods are consistent with those of Fan et al. [22]. Independent samples t-tests were used to determine if there were significant differences in the performance of the attention networks between the groups.

fMRI data preprocessing

The resting-state fMRI data were preprocessed using SPM12 and the DPABI toolbox as described by Yan et al. [88]. The steps were as follows: (1) discarding the initial ten images from each participant to stabilize the magnetic field and ensure participant adaptation, (2) correcting for slice timing and head motion using the default motion correction procedures. (participants exhibiting head motion exceeding 2 mm or 2 degrees were excluded), (3) BET and T1 coregistration to the functional images, (4) regression of nuisance variables including the Friston-24 motion parameters and three other confounding signals (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and global signals), (5) spatial normalization to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space and resampling to 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 resolution, (6) spatial smoothing with a 4 mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel, (7) removal of linear trends, and (8) temporal band-pass filtering within the frequency range of 0.01–0.08 Hz.

Fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations analysis

In the present study, the fALFF methodology was employed as an alternative to the traditional ALFF approach due to the latter’s susceptibility to signal perturbations from physiological sources unrelated to neural activity [100]. ALFF quantifies the amplitude of low-frequency oscillations in the BOLD signal, typically within the 0.01–0.08 Hz range, which is believed to reflect regional spontaneous neural activity. fALFF is defined as the ratio of ALFF within a specific frequency band (e.g., 0.01–0.08 Hz) to the ALFF calculated across the entire frequency range of the BOLD signal. This normalization process effectively reduces the impact of global signal variations and physiological noise, providing a more specific measure of spontaneous neural activity related to neuroplasticity [101]. The fALFF metric was calculated using DPABI [88]. Group-level analyses were conducted using SPM12 with independent t-tests, controlling for age and gender as covariates. Statistical significance was determined at the voxel level using a False Discovery Rate (FDR) corrected threshold of p < 0.05 to account for multiple comparisons. At the cluster level, significance was determined at p < 0.05 with FDR correction, requiring clusters to consist of at least 50 contiguous voxels.

Seed-based functional connectivity analysis

The seed-based voxel-wise resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) analysis consisted of three steps for five seed regions identified by fALFF analysis: (1) extracting and averaging the time courses of all voxels within each seed region; (2) computing Pearson correlation coefficients between the mean time courses of the seeds and those of all other voxels across the brain; (3) transforming the rsFC maps into z-maps using Fisher's z-transformation to enhance normality and improve statistical analysis quality.

For group-level analyses, SPM12 was used to conduct independent t-tests. Age and gender were included as covariates to control for irrelevant variables. The results were thresholded at p < 0.05, FDR-corrected, both voxel-wise and cluster-wise, with a minimum cluster size of 50 voxels.

Multiple linear regression analysis

Seed-based rsFC analysis indicated that the anterior lobe of the cerebellum might play an important role in higher cognitive functions through its connections with cortical areas. To further verify this hypothesis, we extracted and averaged rsFC values across the brain regions that exhibited differential connectivity with the left anterior lobe of the cerebellum (as determined by an independent-samples t-test with voxel-based FDR correction at α = 0.05 and a minimum cluster size of 50 voxels) in the seed-based FC analysis, and then used these aggregated measures for a multiple linear regression analysis comparing experts and novices. Multiple linear regression analyses were then conducted to ascertain whether the differentially active regions could predict the executive control, orienting, and alerting scores for both expert and novice groups, as well as to evaluate the distinct contributions of rsFC in explaining individual variability. Specifically, each region’s average rsFC value served as a separate predictor in the multiple regression models, with a total of eight predictors. The dependent variables included the executive control, orienting, and alerting scores. To further evaluate the relationship between cerebellar cortical connectivity and professional football training, we conducted a multiple linear regression analysis using training years as the dependent variable and the same rsFC values as independent variables, consistent with the methodology described above.

Our multiple linear regression analyses were performed using OriginLab (OriginLab Corporation, Northampton, MA, USA).

Graph theory analysis

Graph theory provides the ideal framework for the precise mathematical depiction of complex networks. In formal terms, a complex network can be depicted as a graph, represented by G(N, K), where N indicates the total nodes and K represents the edges within the graph G. In present study, nodes (N) representing the brain regions in the Automated Anatomical Labeling (AAL) template which divided the gray matter cortex. Edges (K) representing the functional connectivity strength between nodes. Functional network analysis was conducted across the whole brain, using 116 regions defined by the AAL atlas as network nodes [89]. The BOLD time series of all voxels within each region were extracted and averaged. Subsequently, functional connectivity among all ROI pairs was measured using Pearson correlation coefficients. The correlation coefficients were subsequently standardized to z-scores via Fisher's r-to-z transformation. These matrices represent the strength of the functional connectivity within the whole brain and served as edges for the graph analysis.

For the graph analysis, two types of brain network metrics were calculated using Gretna [86]: (1). The nodal network metrics including betweenness centrality, degree centrality and node efficiency within the whole brain, respectively; (2). The small world parameters including normalized clustering coefficient (λ), small-worldness (σ) and normalized characteristic path length (γ). Network sparsity was used as thresholding method, sparsity is calculated as the fraction of the actual number of edges over the highest possible number of edges within a network. In networks that share an equal number of nodes, a sparsity threshold is applied to maintain an identical edge count across all networks, achieved by setting a threshold based on the unique connectivity strength for each subject. This method permits the assessment of the comparative organizational structure of networks [30]. Threshold sequence was calculated based on the existing literature [39, 87]. Network type was set to binary, and the negative correlation coefficients were set to zero.

Then, independent samples T-tests were used to compare brain network indices between experts and novices. The results of the nodal network indices were corrected using FDR correction (α = 0.05).

Results

Behavioral results

The results of independent samples T-test (Fig. 2) showed there were no significant differences for executive control scores(t(110) = 0.594, p = 0.123, Cohen’s d = 0.296). The orienting scores of experts were significantly higher than those of novices. (t(110) = 2.022, p = 0.046, Cohen’s d = 0.385). However, the alerting scores of experts were significantly lower than those of novices. (t(110) = −2.132, p = 0.043, Cohen’s d = −0.405).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Behavioral results of ANT. The efficiency of attention networks, calculated using the subtraction method by Fan et al. [22]. A Executive control score; B orienting score; C alerting score. *p < 0.05

Fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations analysis

The independent samples T-test on the fALFF z-score maps revealed significantly higher fALFF values in experts within the bilateral anterior cerebellar lobes, bilateral insula, and left superior temporal gyrus (FDR-corrected α = 0.05, cluster size > 50; see Fig. 3 and Table 1 for details).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Differences in fALFF between football athletes and non-athletes. The red area indicates that football experts have significantly higher fALFF values in this brain region compared to football novices (FDR-corrected α = 0.05)

Table 1 fALFF differences between football athletes and non-athletes

Seed-based functional connectivity analysis

Seed-based rsFC analysis, utilizing regions identified in the fALFF analysis, was conducted to examine the differences in intrinsic connectivity between experts and novices. Figures and Tables depict the T maps from the seed-based rsFC analysis. For the left anterior cerebellar lobe seed (Fig. 4), experts demonstrated enhanced correlations with clusters in the right supramarginal gyrus, left precuneus, right posterior cerebellar lobe, left middle temporal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, angular gyrus, left superior frontal gyrus, left posterior cerebellar lobe, and bilateral precentral gyrus. For the left superior temporal gyrus seed (Fig. 5), experts showed increased correlation with clusters in the left anterior cerebellar lobe and left inferior frontal gyrus (FDR-corrected α = 0.05, cluster size > 50; see Table 2 for details). For the right cerebellum anterior lobe, left insula, and right insula seed regions, no significantly different clusters were identified between experts and novices.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Functional connectivity differences between experts and novices using the left anterior cerebellar lobe as the seed region

Fig. 5
figure 5

Functional connectivity differences between experts and novices using the left superior temporal gyrus as the seed region

Table 2 Functional connectivity differences between experts and novices

Multiple linear regression analysis

The results of the multiple linear regression analysis, summarized in the Table 3, revealed that the differences in rsFC between the groups exhibit strong predictive power for orienting scores in the expert group (adjusted R2 = 0.178, F = 2.303, p = 0.039). However, such predictive power was not observed in the novice group (adjusted R2 = − 0.001, F = 0.995, p = 0.451). As shown in Fig. 6, the cerebellar-cortical connectivity can predict the attention orientation scores in the expert group but cannot predict the attention orientation scores in the novice group. No severe multicollinearity was observed in either group, with tolerance values above 0.2 and VIFs below 5 for all eight covariates [37]. Additionally, no significant predictors were identified for either the expert or novice groups (Supplementary Tables S2 and S3 for detailed information).

Table 3 Model summary of linear regression models with rsFC
Fig. 6
figure 6

Performance of the multiple linear regression model from the rsFC maps on orienting scores. A Multiple linear regression using rsFC significantly predicted experts’ orienting scores (adjusted R2 = 0.178, p = 0.039). B Multiple linear regression using rsFC did not significantly predict novices’ orienting scores (adjusted R2 = − 0.001, p = 0.451). C The scatter plot displaying the relationship between the regression standardized predicted values and the orienting scores for experts D and for novices

As shown in Fig. 7, the multiple linear regression analysis revealed that rsFC values significantly predicted training years (adjusted R2 = 0.279, F = 3.327, p = 0.005). The coefficients of the predictive factors are presented in the supplementary materials. The cerebellum-precuneus connectivity (β = − 0.438, p = 0.011) and cerebellum-MTG connectivity (β = 0.468, p = 0.024) identified as significant predictors.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Performance of the multiple linear regression model derived from rsFC maps on experts’ training years. A Multiple linear regression using rsFC significantly predicted experts’ training years (adjusted R2 = 0.178, p = 0.005). B The scatter plot displaying the relationship between the regression standardized predicted values and the training years for experts

Graph theoretical analysis

For the nodal network metrics, independent samples t-tests were conducted for each attribute, followed by FDR multiple comparison correction (α = 0.05). The results showed that three nodal regions differed significantly between experts and novices. For the right cerebellar lobule III (Cerebelum_3_R) node, experts showed higher BC (t = 4.283, p < 0.001) and node efficiency (t = 3.363, p = 0.001) compared to novices. For the left dorsal cingulate gyrus (DCG.L) node, experts showed lower DC (t = − 3.644, p < 0.001) and node efficiency (t = − 3.645, p < 0.001) compared to novices. For the left supplementary motor area (SMA.L) node, experts showed lower node efficiency (t = − 3.332, p = 0.001) compared to novices.

For the small-world parameters, both athletes and non-athletes exhibited economic “small-world” properties (λ ≈ 1, γ > > 1). This indicates that their networks achieve global information processing efficiency comparable to a matched random network, while also demonstrating enhanced local information integration efficiency and fault tolerance [87]. The results of two-sample t-tests showed no differences between experts and novices in the three small-world parameters.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the differences in brain function and attentional abilities between professional football athletes and novices using resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging and the Attention Network Test. We found that football experts and novices exhibited differences in fALFF, intrinsic functional connectivity, and topological properties of brain networks. Furthermore, significant differences were observed between football experts and novices in aspects of attentional orienting and alerting. More importantly, functional connectivity differences observed in the connectivity analysis significantly predicted attentional orienting scores and training years in the expert group. Taken together, our results indicated significant neuroplastic changes and attentional alterations in athletes, suggesting that the cerebellum might play a crucial role in orienting attention functions.

The behavioral results from the ANT revealed that football experts exhibited superior orienting attention but weaker alerting functions compared to novices, with no difference in executive control attention. These three ANT components represent distinct attentional processes. The alerting attention represents the efficiency of achieving and maintaining a vigilant state in response to an impending stimulus. The orienting attention indicates the ability to shift attentional focus toward relevant spatial or sensory cues. The executive control attention measures the capacity to resolve conflicts between competing responses [44]. Previous meta-analyses have indicated that football training may enhance attentional abilities in children and adolescents [43]. For example, Rahimi et al. [60] reported that soccer players demonstrated faster response times in the alerting and valid orienting cue conditions of the ANT. Consistent with these findings, the present study showed that football experts exhibited superior orienting attention. However, we did not find that football experts possessed enhanced executive control attention, which is inconsistent with some previous studies [1, 52, 84]. For example, Moratal et al. [52] found that, compared to non-athletes, soccer players exhibited overall faster responses and better executive control attention on the ANT. Moreover. our finding diverges from some previous studies that reported enhanced alerting functions in athletes [60, 81]. One possible reason for the lower alerting scores in experts is that their training emphasizes quick, automatic responses to familiar stimuli, potentially reducing the need for heightened alertness. The enhanced orienting function in experts may be attributed to the specific demands of football, which requires rapid and precise attention shifts to dynamic and spatially distributed stimuli [40]. In summary, our study suggests that the relationship between motor training and attentional functions is multifaceted. While football training appears to enhance orienting attention, it does not uniformly improve all components of the attentional functions. Therefore, further research is needed to elucidate the underlying mechanisms and to explore how different types of training may differentially impact various aspects of attention.

Our findings were consistent with previous research showing that long-term motor training induces both structural and functional plasticity in the brain [40, 46, 89, 91, 95, 96, 98]. Specifically, we observed higher fALFF values in football experts within the bilateral anterior cerebellar lobes, bilateral insula, and left superior temporal gyrus. The anterior cerebellum, traditionally known for its role in motor control and coordination, has gained attention for its involvement in cognitive performance and attentional processes [5, 66]. The insula, located deep within the lateral sulcus, is an important node of the fronto-parietal attention network [24, 74], and is closely related to attentional function [77]. Increased neural activity in the insula might facilitate the integration of sensory information and the allocation of attentional resources to salient stimuli. The superior temporal gyrus is known for its role in auditory processing and language comprehension [51, 92]. fALFF reflects the amplitude of low-frequency oscillations (0.01–0.08 Hz) within a specific frequency range, which are believed to reflect the intensity of regional spontaneous brain activity [100]. The enhanced fALFF observed in football athletes indicates increased spontaneous neuronal activity in these regions, suggesting that sports training may induce adaptive neural changes, which is consistent with previous studies [32, 59, 97]. Moreover, the present study reveals that the effects of sports training on the brain are widespread, influencing not only motor control related regions but also areas linked to higher cognitive functions, such as the insula. These findings provide valuable insights into the mechanisms underlying neuroplastic changes induced by physical training.

Seed-based functional connectivity analysis further clarified the neural underpinnings of these differences. Football experts showed increased connectivity between the left anterior cerebellar lobe and various cortical regions, including the right supramarginal gyrus, left precuneus, left superior frontal gyrus, bilateral posterior cerebellar lobe, and bilateral precentral gyrus. Additionally, the functional connectivity between the left STG and the left anterior cerebellum was stronger in experts than in novices. The supramarginal gyrus is considered to be associated with word processing [55, 65, 69]. The precuneus is a core node of the Default Mode Network and is associated with higher cognitive functions, including attention [13, 16, 42, 78]. Furthermore, previous studies showed that the precuneus was activated during the execution of attentional orienting tasks [26, 54, 93]. The superior frontal gyrus contributes to higher cognitive functions, particularly working memory [7, 38, 45]. The posterior parts of the cerebellum are involved in higher cognitive modulation [5, 63]. The precentral gyrus is widely recognized for its crucial role in movement execution [33, 62]. The precentral gyrus is also thought to play a key role in higher cognitive processes, such as attention, motor learning, movement imagery and language [6, 83, 99]. Previous research utilizing transfer techniques and neuroimaging has revealed extensive connections between the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex [19, 35, 36, 70, 71]. These connections are functionally implicated in the coordination of motor and cognitive behaviors [10, 23, 61]. Furthermore, previous studies have found that motor training can modulate the connectivity between the cerebellum and motor-related cortical regions [47, 75, 76]. For example, Mehrkanoon et al. [47] found enhanced connectivity within the cerebellum and between the cerebellum and motor cortex following motor training, as revealed by source-reconstructed electroencephalography. Consistent with previous studies, the current study found that football athletes exhibited stronger functional connectivity within the cerebellum and between the cerebellum and motor-related cortex (bilateral precentral gyrus) compared to novices. Furthermore, we found that long-term sports training also enhanced connectivity between the cerebellum and broader brain regions, including the precuneus and supramarginal gyrus. The findings suggest that long-term motor training may induce plastic changes in cerebellar-cortical connectivity, and these adaptive changes could potentially regulate cognitive abilities such as attention and language. Furthermore, the multiple linear regression analysis examining the relationship between rsFC and years of football training revealed that enhanced rsFC significantly predicted athletes’ training years, with cerebellum-precuneus connectivity and cerebellum-MTG connectivity identified as significant predictors. These findings provide additional evidence that motor training induces plastic changes in cerebellar-cortical connectivity.

More importantly, we found the enhanced functional connectivity can significantly predicted attentional orienting scores of experts, a phenomenon not observed in the novices. Previous studies have emphasized the crucial role of the cerebellum in attentional functions [2, 3, 5, 15, 63, 66]. For example, Brissenden et al. [8] found that visual attention tasks strongly engage the cerebellum, and cerebellum-to-cortex functional connectivity significantly predicted cortical activation patterns during task performance. The current study further demonstrated that the cerebellum may participate in attentional orienting functions through extensive crossed cerebrocerebellar connections. Additionally, this involvement was modulated by long-term professional football training. Furthermore, previous studies primarily linked the posterior lobe of the cerebellum to attentional processes [2, 5], [64]. However, our findings suggest that the anterior cerebellum may also contribute to attentional processes. This observation may be understood within the broader context of the relationship between attention and motor control. Attention and motor control are closely intertwined, as rely on shared neural resources and overlapping networks [14, 20]. Effective motor control often demands precise allocation of attentional resources to guide movements and adapt to dynamic environments ([18]; [53]. Given the anterior cerebellum’s established role in motor coordination [72], [66] and its connectivity with cortical areas implicated in attention, it is plausible that this region supports attentional processes indirectly through its involvement in motor-cognitive integration. Long-term professional football training may further enhance this integration, facilitating the adaptation of attentional functions to task-specific demands. Our finding highlights the need for a more nuanced understanding of how the cerebellum contributes to the interplay between motor and cognitive processes.

Graph theoretical analysis emphasized the impact of athletic training on neural plasticity, suggesting that football training could reorganize functional connectivity patterns within the brain. Experts exhibited higher betweenness centrality and node efficiency in the Cerebellum_3_R node, a part of the anterior lobe of the cerebellum, indicating enhanced integration and coordination of neural information within this region. The results of graph theoretical analysis, consistent with fALFF and functional connectivity analyses, indicate the impact of physical training on cerebellar function. Conversely, lower degree centrality and node efficiency in the DCG.L node, a part of the dorsal cingulate gyrus, may reflect a reorganization prioritizing essential connections, enhancing processing efficiency for rapid attentional shifts and decision-making in football. Additionally, both athletes and non-athletes displayed economic “small-world” properties without significant differences between groups. This suggests that while soccer training enhances specific nodal efficiencies, it does not alter the fundamental small-world architecture of brain networks. Overall, these findings suggest that long-term professional football training leads to significant neuroplastic changes, particularly in regions associated with motor and cognitive functions.

Conclusion

In summary, this study highlights the significant impact of long-term professional football training on neuroplasticity and attentional functions. Our findings demonstrated that football athletes exhibit enhanced functional connectivity between the cerebellum and various brain regions involved in higher cognitive processes, compared to novices. Specifically, the cerebellum’s involvement in attentional orienting functions appears to be modulated by extensive crossed cerebrocerebellar connections. Moreover, graph theoretical analysis indicated that football training enhances integration and coordination of neural information, particularly in the cerebellum. These findings reveal a neurobiological basis for the correlation between neuroplasticity and attentional plasticity, highlighting how professional football training leads to significant neuroplastic changes and enhanced attentional functions.

Limitation

This study selected professional football athletes and used university students without football experience as a control group. Despite balancing for age and educational background between the groups, we must acknowledge that this study cannot determine whether changes in brain function and attentional abilities in football players are due to innate factors, environmental influences, or a combination of both. Future research could incorporate longitudinal designs to track neural and cognitive changes over time, providing insights into causal relationships. Another limitation of this study is the lack of control for cardiovascular fitness variables, such as heart rate or blood flow, which could affect BOLD signal interpretation and resting-state fMRI results. Future studies should include these measures to improve accuracy in functional connectivity assessments.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Alesi M, Bianco A, Luppina G, Palma A, Pepi A. Improving children’s coordinative skills and executive functions: the effects of a football exercise program. Percept Mot Skills. 2016;122(1):27–46. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/0031512515627527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Allen G, Buxton RB, Wong EC, Courchesne E. Attentional activation of the cerebellum independent of motor involvement. Science. 1997;275(5308):1940–3. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.275.5308.1940.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Akshoomoff NA, Courchesne E. ERP evidence for a shifting attention deficit in patients with damage to the cerebellum. J Cogn Neurosci. 1994;6(4):388–99. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1162/jocn.1994.6.4.388.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Amad A, Seidman J, Draper SB, Bruchhage MMK, Lowry RG, Wheeler J, Robertson A, Williams SCR, Smith MS. Motor learning induces plasticity in the resting brain—drumming up a connection. Cereb Cortex. 2016. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhw048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier P, Paul D, Mariën P. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110:763–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.clineuro.2008.05.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bhattacharjee S, Kashyap R, Abualait T, Chen S-HA, Yoo W-K, Bashir S. The role of primary motor cortex: more than movement execution. J Motor Behav. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/00222895.2020.1738992.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. du Boisgueheneuc F, Levy R, Volle E, Seassau M, Duffau H, Kinkingnehun S, Samson Y, Zhang S, Dubois B. Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans: a lesion study. Brain. 2006;129(12):3315–28. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/brain/awl244.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Brissenden JA, Levin EJ, Osher DE, Halko MA, Somers DC. Functional evidence for a cerebellar node of the dorsal attention network. J Neurosci. 2016;36(22):6083–96. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0344-16.2016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Buckner RL, Sepulcre J, Talukdar T, Krienen FM, Liu H, Hedden T, Andrews-Hanna JR, Sperling RA, Johnson KA. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2009;29(6):1860–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5062-08.2009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Buckner RL. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron. 2013;80(3):807–15. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.044.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Calmels C. Neural correlates of motor expertise: extensive motor training and cortical changes. Brain Res. 2020;1739:146323. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.brainres.2019.146323.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Cantou P, Platel H, Desgranges B, Groussard M How motor, cognitive and musical expertise shapes the brain: Focus on fMRI and EEG resting-state functional connectivity. J Chem Neuroanat. 2018;89:60–68. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jchemneu.2017.08.003.

  13. Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. The precuneus: a review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain. 2006;129(3):564–83. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/brain/awl004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–15. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/nrn755.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Courchesne E, Townsend J, Akshoomoff NA, Saitoh O, Yeung-Courchesne R, Lincoln AJ, James HE, Haas RH, Schreibman L, Lau L. Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108(5):848–65. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0735-7044.108.5.848.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Dadario NB, Sughrue ME. The functional role of the precuneus. Brain. 2023;146(9):3598–607. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/brain/awad181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dayan E, Cohen LG. Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron. 2011;72(3):443–54. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995;18(1):193–222.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Dum RP, Strick PL. An unfolded map of the cerebellar dentate nucleus and its projections to the cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2003;89(1):634–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1152/jn.00626.2002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Engel AK, Maye A, Kurthen M, König P. Where’s the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(5):202–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Fan J, Gu X, Guise KG, Liu X, Fossella J, Wang H, Posner MI. Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks. Brain Cogn. 2009;70(2):209–20. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Fan J, Mccandliss B, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner M. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14:340–7. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1162/089892902317361886.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fernandez L, Rogasch NC, Do M, Clark G, Major BP, Teo W-P, Byrne LK, Enticott PG. Cerebral cortical activity following non-invasive cerebellar stimulation—a systematic review of combined TMS and EEG studies. Cerebellum. 2020;19(2):309–35. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s12311-019-01093-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2005;102(27):9673–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1073/pnas.0504136102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Friston KJ, Frith CD, Liddle PF, Frackowiak RSJ. Functional connectivity: the principal-component analysis of large (PET) data sets. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 1993;13(1):5–14. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/jcbfm.1993.4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Galashan D, Siemann J. Differences and similarities for spatial and feature-based selective attentional orienting. Front Neurosci. 2017. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnins.2017.00283.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Guo CC, Kurth F, Zhou J, Mayer EA, Eickhoff SB, Kramer JH, Seeley WW. One-year test–retest reliability of intrinsic connectivity network fMRI in older adults. NeuroImage 2012;61(4):1471–83. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.027.

  28. Hardwick RM, Rottschy C, Miall RC, Eickhoff SB. A quantitative meta-analysis and review of motor learning in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2013;67:283–97. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Hortobágyi T, Vetrovsky T, Balbim GM, Sorte Silva NCB, Manca A, Deriu F, Kolmos M, Kruuse C, Liu-Ambrose T, Radák Z, Váczi M, Johansson H, dos Santos PCR, Franzén E, Granacher U. The impact of aerobic and resistance training intensity on markers of neuroplasticity in health and disease. Ageing Res Rev. 2022;80:101698. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.arr.2022.101698.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. He Y, Dagher A, Chen Z, Charil A, Zijdenbos A, Worsley K, Evans A. Impaired small-world efficiency in structural cortical networks in multiple sclerosis associated with white matter lesion load. Brain 2009;132(12):3366–79. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/brain/awp089.

  31. Hüttermann S, Memmert D, Simons DJ. The size and shape of the attentional “spotlight” varies with differences in sports expertise. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2014;20(2):147–57. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/xap0000012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Jin X, Chen S, Qi Y, Zhou Q, Wang J, Wang Y, Zhou C. Differential resting-state brain characteristics of skeleton athletes and non-athletes: a preliminary resting-state fMRI study. Brain Sci. 2024;14(10): 10. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3390/brainsci14101016.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Kakei S, Hoffman DS, Strick PL. Muscle and movement representations in the primary motor cortex. Science. 1999;285(5436):2136–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.285.5436.2136.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Kang N. Increased cerebellar gray matter volume in athletes: a voxel-wise coordinate-based meta-analysis. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2023;94(3):597–608. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1080/02701367.2022.2026285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Kelly RM, Strick PL. Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman primate. J Neurosci. 2003;23(23):8432–44. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-23-08432.2003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Keser Z, Hasan KM, Mwangi BI, Kamali A, Ucisik-Keser FE, Riascos RF, Yozbatiran N, Francisco GE, Narayana PA. Diffusion tensor imaging of the human cerebellar pathways and their interplay with cerebral macrostructure. Front Neuroanat. 2015. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnana.2015.00041.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Kutner MH, Nachtsheim CJ, Neter J, Li W. Applied linear statistical models. New York: McGraw-hill; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Li W, Qin W, Liu H, Fan L, Wang J, Jiang T, Yu C. Subregions of the human superior frontal gyrus and their connections. Neuroimage. 2013;78:46–58. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Liao X, Yuan L, Zhao T, Dai Z, Shu N, Xia M, Yang Y, Evans A, He Y. Spontaneous functional network dynamics and associated structural substrates in the human brain. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00478.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Lu Q, Li P, Wu Q, Liu X, Wu Y. Efficiency and enhancement in attention networks of elite shooting and archery athletes. Front Psychol. 2021. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.638822.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Lv H, Wang Z, Tong E, Williams LM, Zaharchuk G, Zeineh M, Goldstein-Piekarski AN, Ball TM, Liao C, Wintermark M. Resting-State Functional MRI: Everything That Nonexperts Have Always Wanted to Know. Am J Neuroradiol. 2018;39(8):1390–99. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3174/ajnr.A5527.

  42. Mahayana IT, Tcheang L, Chen C-Y, Juan C-H, Muggleton NG. The precuneus and visuospatial attention in near and far space: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Brain Stimul. 2014;7(5):673–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.brs.2014.06.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Mao F, Yin A, Zhao S, Fang Q. Effects of football training on cognitive performance in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Front Psychol. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1449612.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Markett S, Reuter M, Montag C, Voigt G, Lachmann B, Rudorf S, Elger CE, Weber B. Assessing the function of the fronto-parietal attention network: insights from resting-state fMRI and the attentional network test. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(4):1700–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/hbm.22285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Martino J, Gabarrós A, Deus J, Juncadella M, Acebes JJ, Torres A, Pujol J. Intrasurgical mapping of complex motor function in the superior frontal gyrus. Neuroscience. 2011;179:131–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.01.047.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Maudrich T, Hähner S, Kenville R, Ragert P. Somatosensory-evoked potentials as a marker of functional neuroplasticity in athletes: a systematic review. Front Physiol. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fphys.2021.821605.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Mehrkanoon S, Boonstra TW, Breakspear M, Hinder M, Summers JJ. Upregulation of cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity after motor learning. Neuroimage. 2016;128:252–63. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.12.052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Memmert D, Simons DJ, Grimme T. The relationship between visual attention and expertise in sports. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2009;10(1):146–51. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.psychsport.2008.06.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Meng F-W, Yao Z-F, Chang EC, Chen Y-L. Team sport expertise shows superior stimulus-driven visual attention and motor inhibition. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(5): e0217056. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0217056.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Mercado E. Neural and cognitive plasticity: from maps to minds. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(1):109–37. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Mesgarani N, Cheung C, Johnson K, Chang EF. Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science. 2014;343(6174):1006–10. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1126/science.1245994.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Moratal C, Lupiáñez J, Ballester R, Huertas F. Deliberate soccer practice modulates attentional functioning in children. Front Psychol. 2020. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00761.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Moore T, Zirnsak M. Neural Mechanisms of Selective Visual Attention. Annu Rev Psychol. 2017:47–72. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033400.

  54. Nobre AC, Coull JT, Maquet P, Frith CD, Vandenberghe R, Mesulam MM. Orienting attention to locations in perceptual versus mental representations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(3):363–73. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1162/089892904322926700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Oberhuber M, Hope TMH, Seghier ML, Parker Jones O, Prejawa S, Green DW, Price CJ. Four functionally distinct regions in the left supramarginal gyrus support word processing. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26(11):4212–26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhw251.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Patel R, Spreng RN, Turner GR. Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27(3):187–99. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1545968312461718.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Posner MI, Petersen SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Poysophon P, Rao AL. Neurocognitive deficits associated with ADHD in athletes: a systematic review. Sports Health. 2018;10(4):317–26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1177/1941738117751387.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Qi Y, Zhao M, Yan Z, Jia X, Wang Y. Altered spontaneous regional brain activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex and visual area of expert table tennis athletes. Brain Imaging Behav. 2024;18(3):529–38. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s11682-023-00841-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Rahimi A, Roberts SD, Baker JR, Wojtowicz M. Attention and executive control in varsity athletes engaging in strategic and static sports. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(4): e0266933. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0266933.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Ramnani N, Toni I, Josephs O, Ashburner J, Passingham RE. Learning- and expectation-related changes in the human brain during motor learning. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84(6):3026–35. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1152/jn.2000.84.6.3026.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:393–415. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.393.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Schmahmann JD. The cerebellum and cognition. Neurosci Lett. 2019;688:62–75. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.005.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Schmahmann JD. Disorders of the Cerebellum: Ataxia, Dysmetria of Thought, and the Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome. J Neuropsychiatr Clin Neurosci. 2004;16(3):367–78. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1176/jnp.16.3.367.

  65. Sliwinska MWW, Khadilkar M, Campbell-Ratcliffe J, Quevenco F, Devlin JT. Early and sustained supramarginal gyrus contributions to phonological processing. Front Psychol. 2012. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  66. Sokolov AA, Miall RC, Ivry RB. The cerebellum: adaptive prediction for movement and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2017;21(5):313–32. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.tics.2017.02.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Sporns O, Chialvo DR, Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. Organization, development and function of complex brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(9):418–25. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Sporns O, Honey CJ, Kötter R. Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS ONE. 2007;2(10): e1049. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0001049.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Stoeckel C, Gough PM, Watkins KE, Devlin JT. Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex. 2009;45(9):1091–6. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Stoodley CJ. The cerebellum and cognition: evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum. 2012;11(2):352–65. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s12311-011-0260-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Evidence for topographic organization in the cerebellum of motor control versus cognitive and affective processing. Lang, Cognit Cerebellum: Grapp Enigm. 2010;46(7):831–44. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cortex.2009.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD. Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. NeuroImage 2009;44(2):489–501. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.039.

  73. Taubert M, Lohmann G, Margulies DS, Villringer A, Ragert P. Long-term effects of motor training on resting-state networks and underlying brain structure. Neuroimage. 2011;57(4):1492–8. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Toro R, Fox PT, Paus T. Functional coactivation map of the human brain. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18(11):2553–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhn014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Tzvi E, Gajiyeva L, Bindel L, Hartwigsen G, Classen J. Coherent theta oscillations in the cerebellum and supplementary motor area mediate visuomotor adaptation. Neuroimage. 2022;251:118985. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118985.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Tzvi E, Koeth F, Karabanov AN, Siebner HR, Krämer UM. Cerebellar—Premotor cortex interactions underlying visuomotor adaptation. Neuroimage. 2020;220:117142. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117142.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hébert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O. Structure and function of the human insula. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;34(4):300–6. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Utevsky AV, Smith DV, Huettel SA. Precuneus is a functional core of the default-mode network. J Neurosci. 2014;34(3):932–40. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4227-13.2014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Vahdat S, Darainy M, Milner TE, Ostry DJ. Functionally specific changes in resting-state sensorimotor networks after motor learning. J Neurosci. 2011;31(47):16907–15. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2737-11.2011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Van Den Heuvel MP, Hulshoff Pol HE. Exploring the brain network: a review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20(8):519–34. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.03.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Verburgh L, Scherder EJA, van Lange PAM, Oosterlaan J. Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(3): e91254. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0091254.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Voss MW, Kramer AF, Basak C, Prakash RS, Roberts B. Are expert athletes ‘expert’ in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl Cogn Psychol. 2010;24(6):812–26. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1002/acp.1588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Vukovic N, Feurra M, Shpektor A, Myachykov A, Shtyrov Y. Primary motor cortex functionally contributes to language comprehension: an online rTMS study. Neuropsychologia. 2017;96:222–9. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Wang C-H, Yang C-T, Moreau D, Muggleton NG. Motor expertise modulates neural oscillations and temporal dynamics of cognitive control. Neuroimage. 2017;158:260–70. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Wang J. Graph-based network analysis of resting-state functional MRI. Front Syst Neurosci. 2010. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnsys.2010.00016.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  86. Wang J, Wang X, Xia M, Liao X, Evans A, He Y. GRETNA: a graph theoretical network analysis toolbox for imaging connectomics. Front Human Neurosci. 2015. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Watts DJ, Strogatz SH. Collective dynamics of ‘small-world’ networks. Nature. 1998;393:440.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Yan C-G, Wang X-D, Zuo X-N, Zang Y-F. DPABI: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics. 2016;14(3):339–51. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Yan S, Zhao G, Zhang Q, Liu L, Bai X, Jin H. Altered resting-state brain function in endurance athletes. Cereb Cortex. 2024;34(3):bhae076. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1093/cercor/bhae076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Yao Z-F, Sligte IG, Moreau D, Hsieh S, Yang C-T, Ridderinkhof KR, Muggleton NG, Wang C-H. The brains of elite soccer players are subject to experience-dependent alterations in white matter connectivity. Cortex. 2020;132:79–91. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.cortex.2020.07.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Yarrow K, Brown P, Krakauer JW. Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(8):585–96. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/nrn2672.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Yi HG, Leonard MK, Chang EF. The encoding of speech sounds in the superior temporal gyrus. Neuron. 2019;102(6):1096–110. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuron.2019.04.023.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  93. Yin X, Zhao L, Xu J, Evans AC, Fan L, Ge H, Tang Y, Khundrakpam B, Wang J, Liu S. Anatomical substrates of the alerting, orienting and executive control components of attention: focus on the posterior parietal lobe. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(11): e50590. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1371/journal.pone.0050590.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  94. Yao Z-F, Sligte IG, Ridderinkhof R. Olympic team rowers and team swimmers show altered functional brain activation during working memory and action inhibition. Neuropsychologia. 2024;203:108974. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2024.108974.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Zhang J-P, Zhu P, Cai Z, Xing X-X, Wu J-J, Zheng M-X, Hua X-Y, Gong B-M, Xu J-G. Sports promote brain evolution: a resting-state fMRI study of volleyball athlete. Front Sports Act Living. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.21203/rs.3.rs-3889788/v1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Zhang K, Cao C, Wang Y, Zhang D. Brain structure and function differences across varying levels of endurance training: a cross-sectional study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2024. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnhum.2024.1503094.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Zhang K, Jan Y-K, Liu Y, Zhao T, Zhang L, Liu R, Liu J, Cao C. Exercise intensity and brain plasticity: what’s the difference of brain structural and functional plasticity characteristics between elite aerobic and anaerobic athletes? Front Human Neurosci. 2022. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fnhum.2022.757522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Zhang K, Liu Y, Liu J, Liu R, Cao C. Detecting structural and functional neuroplasticity in elite ice-skating athletes. Hum Mov Sci. 2021;78:102795. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.humov.2021.102795.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Zhao M, Marino M, Samogin J, Swinnen SP, Mantini D. Hand, foot and lip representations in primary sensorimotor cortex: a high-density electroencephalography study. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):19464. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1038/s41598-019-55369-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Zou Q-H, Zhu C-Z, Yang Y, Zuo X-N, Long X-Y, Cao Q-J, Wang Y-F, Zang Y-F. An improved approach to detection of amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) for resting-state fMRI: fractional ALFF. J Neurosci Methods. 2008;172(1):137–41. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.04.012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  101. Zuo X-N, Di Martino A, Kelly C, Shehzad ZE, Gee DG, Klein DF, Castellanos FX, Biswal BB, Milham MP. The oscillating brain: complex and reliable. Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1432–45. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to express our deepest gratitude to our colleagues and mentors at Shanghai University of Sport for their continuous support, insightful comments, and hard questions, all of which have significantly contributed to the improvement of this research work. This work was supported by the grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.32371129).

Funding

This work was supported by the grants from National Natural Science Foundation of China (No.32371129).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JW was responsible for data curation, conceptualization, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, writing the original draft, reviewing and editing the manuscript, and project administration. SG contributed to data curation. JTwas involved in investigation and data curation. HH contributed to reviewing and editing the manuscript. CZ provided resources, reviewed and edited the manuscript, and supervised the project.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Hao Hong or Chenglin Zhou.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All experimental procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of Shanghai University of Sport, China (No. 102772022RT082, Data: 13/09/2022).

Consent for publication

Informed consent was obtained in written form from all participants prior to their involvement in the study.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wang, J., Gao, S., Tian, J. et al. The role of cerebellar-cortical connectivity in modulating attentional abilities: insight from football athletes. Behav Brain Funct 21, 9 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12993-025-00272-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12993-025-00272-3

Keywords